Results for 'cognitive evidence'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  28
    Primate cognition: evidence for the ethical treatment of primates.Richard W. Byrne - 1999 - In Francine L. Dolins (ed.), Attitudes to Animals: Views in Animal Welfare. Cambridge University Press. pp. 114--125.
  2. Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training☆.Fadel Zeidan, Susan K. Johnson, Bruce J. Diamond, Zhanna David & Paula Goolkasian - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (2):597-605.
    Although research has found that long-term mindfulness meditation practice promotes executive functioning and the ability to sustain attention, the effects of brief mindfulness meditation training have not been fully explored. We examined whether brief meditation training affects cognition and mood when compared to an active control group. After four sessions of either meditation training or listening to a recorded book, participants with no prior meditation experience were assessed with measures of mood, verbal fluency, visual coding, and working memory. Both interventions (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   59 citations  
  3.  31
    Spatial language facilitates spatial cognition: Evidence from children who lack language input.Dedre Gentner, Asli Özyürek, Özge Gürcanli & Susan Goldin-Meadow - 2013 - Cognition 127 (3):318-330.
    No categories
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  4.  24
    Culture, Class and Cognition: Evidence from Italy.Nicola Knight & Richard Nisbett - 2007 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (3-4):283-291.
    East Asians have been found to reason in relatively holistic fashion and Americans in relatively analytic fashion. It has been proposed that these cognitive differences are the result of social practices that encourage interdependence for Asians and independence for Americans. If so, cognitive differences might be found even across regions that are geographically close. We compared performance on a categorization task of relatively interdependent southern Italians and relatively independent northern Italians and found the former to reason in a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  5.  65
    Plasticity in high-order cognition: Evidence of dissociation in aphasia.Rosemary Varley - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (2):171-172.
    High-order constructs such as intelligence result from the interaction of numerous processing systems, one of which is language. However, in determining the role of language in intelligence, attention must be paid to evidence from lesion studies and, in particular, evidence of dissociation of functions where high-order cognition can be demonstrated in face of profound aphasia.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  35
    Composite body movements modulate numerical cognition: evidence from the motion-numerical compatibility effect.Xiaorong Cheng, Hui Ge, Deljfina Andoni, Xianfeng Ding & Zhao Fan - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  7.  8
    Digital Learning As Enhanced Learning Processing? Cognitive Evidence for New insight of Smart Learning.Dina Di Giacomo, Jessica Ranieri & Pilar Lacasa - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  6
    Editorial: Revisiting the Effectiveness of Transcranial Direct Current Brain Stimulation for Cognition: Evidence, Challenges, and Open Questions.Evangelia G. Chrysikou, Marian E. Berryhill, Marom Bikson & H. Branch Coslett - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
  9. The cognitive consequences of forces confabulation: evidence from studies of eyewitness suggestibility.Quin M. Chrobak & Zaragoza & S. Maria - 2009 - In William Hirstein (ed.), Confabulation: Views From Neuroscience, Psychiatry, Psychology and Philosophy. Oxford University Press.
  10. Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for “top-down” effects.Chaz Firestone & Brian J. Scholl - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:1-72.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   192 citations  
  11. A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: evidence from functional neuroimaging.Sean Spence - 2006 - In Semir Zeki & Oliver Goodenough (eds.), Law and the Brain. Oxford University Press.
  12.  15
    Animal Cognition: Theory and Evidence: Review of Species of Mind: The Philosophy and Biology of Cognitive Ethology by Colin Allen and Marc Bekoff. [REVIEW]William Robinson - 1998 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 4.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  50
    The Cognitive‐Evolutionary Model of Surprise: A Review of the Evidence[REVIEW]Rainer Reisenzein, Gernot Horstmann & Achim Schützwohl - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (1):50-74.
    Research on surprise relevant to the cognitive-evolutionary model of surprise proposed by Meyer, Reisenzein, and Schützwohl is reviewed. The majority of the assumptions of the model are found empirically supported. Surprise is evoked by unexpected events and its intensity is determined by the degree if schema-discrepancy, whereas the novelty and the valence of the eliciting events probably do not have an independent effect. Unexpected events cause an automatic interruption of ongoing mental processes that is followed by an attentional shift (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  14.  34
    Empirical Evidence for Extended Cognitive Systems.Luis H. Favela, Mary Jean Amon, Lorena Lobo & Anthony Chemero - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (11):e13060.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 45, Issue 11, November 2021.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  15.  57
    Cognitive templates for religious concepts: cross‐cultural evidence for recall of counter‐intuitive representations.Pascal Boyer & Charles Ramble - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (4):535-564.
    Presents results of free‐recall experiments conducted in France, Gabon and Nepal, to test predictions of a cognitive model of religious concepts. The world over, these concepts include violations of conceptual expectations at the level of domain knowledge (e.g., about ‘animal’ or ‘artifact’ or ‘person’) rather than at the basic level. In five studies we used narratives to test the hypothesis that domain‐level violations are recalled better than other conceptual associations. These studies used material constructed in the same way as (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   98 citations  
  16. Is behavioural flexibility evidence of cognitive complexity? How evolution can inform comparative cognition.Irina Mikhalevich, Russell Powell & Corina Logan - 2017 - Interface Focus 7.
    Behavioural flexibility is often treated as the gold standard of evidence for more sophisticated or complex forms of animal cognition, such as planning, metacognition and mindreading. However, the evidential link between behavioural flexibility and complex cognition has not been explicitly or systematically defended. Such a defence is particularly pressing because observed flexible behaviours can frequently be explained by putatively simpler cognitive mechanisms. This leaves complex cognition hypotheses open to ‘deflationary’ challenges that are accorded greater evidential weight precisely because (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  17.  58
    Evidence for massive redeployment of brain areas in cognitive functions.Michael Anderson - 2006
    sides of the argument. MRH is supported by some case studies of redeployment, and an empirical review of 135..
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  18.  16
    The cognitive consequences of forced fabrication: Evidence from studies of eyewitness suggestibility.Q. Chrobak & Maria S. Zaragoza - 2009 - In William Hirstein (ed.), Confabulation: Views From Neuroscience, Psychiatry, Psychology and Philosophy. Oxford University Press. pp. 67--90.
  19. Cognitive penetrability of the face structural encoding: Electrophysiological evidence.S. Bentin & Y. Golland - 2002 - Cognition 86:1-14.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20. A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: evidence from functional neuroimaging Sean A. Spence*, Mike D. Hunter, Tom FD Farrow, Russell D. Green. [REVIEW]David H. Leung, Catherine J. Hughes & Venkatasubramanian Ganesan - 2006 - In Semir Zeki & Oliver Goodenough (eds.), Law and the Brain. Oxford University Press. pp. 169.
  21. Evidence and the Aim of Cognitive Activity.Nathan Rotenstreich - 1978 - Analecta Husserliana 7:245.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  5
    Evidence and Cognition.Samuel D. Taylor & Jon Williamson - forthcoming - Erkenntnis:1-22.
    Cognitive theorists routinely disagree about the evidence supporting claims in cognitive science. Here, we first argue that some disagreements about evidence in cognitive science are about the evidence available to be drawn upon by cognitive theorists. Then, we show that one’s explanation of why this first kind of disagreement obtains will cohere with one’s theory of evidence. We argue that the best explanation for why cognitive theorists disagree in this way is (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  23.  21
    Cognitive constraints on constituent order: Evidence from elicited pantomime.Matthew L. Hall, Rachel I. Mayberry & Victor S. Ferreira - 2013 - Cognition 129 (1):1-17.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  24.  9
    Cognitive and pragmatic factors in language production: Evidence from source-goal motion events.Monica L. Do, Anna Papafragou & John Trueswell - 2020 - Cognition 205 (C):104447.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  25.  5
    Whence Cognitive Prototypes in Impression Formation?: Some Empirical Evidence for Dialetical Reasoning As a Generative Process.James Lamiell & Patricia Durbeck - 1987 - Journal of Mind and Behavior 8 (2).
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  14
    Unveiling the interplay between evidence, values and cognitive biases. The case of the failure of the AstraZeneca COVID‐19 vaccine.Cristina Amoretti & Elisabetta Lalumera - 2023 - Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice 1.
  27.  81
    Cognitive empathy presupposes self-awareness: Evidence from phylogeny, ontogeny, neuropsychology, and mental illness.Gordon G. Gallup & Steven M. Platek - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (1):36-37.
    We argue that cognitive empathy and other instances of mental state attribution are a byproduct of self-awareness. Evidence is brought to bear on this proposition from comparative psychology, early child development, neuropsychology, and abnormal behavior.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  28. The epistemology of evidence in cognitive neuroscience.William P. Bechtel - forthcoming - In R. Skipper Jr, C. Allen, R. A. Ankeny, C. F. Craver, L. Darden, G. Mikkelson & and R. Richardson (eds.), Philosophy and the Life Sciences: A Reader. MIT Press.
    It is no secret that scientists argue. They argue about theories. But even more, they argue about the evidence for theories. Is the evidence itself trustworthy? This is a bit surprising from the perspective of traditional empiricist accounts of scientific methodology according to which the evidence for scientific theories stems from observation, especially observation with the naked eye. These accounts portray the testing of scientific theories as a matter of comparing the predictions of the theory with the (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  29.  26
    Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia.Michael McCloskey - 1992 - Cognition 44 (1-2):107-157.
  30.  20
    The “cognitive neuroscience revolution” is not a (Kuhnian) revolution. Evidence from scientometrics.Eugenio Petrovich & Marco Viola - 2022 - Rivista Internazionale di Filosofia e Psicologia 13 (2):142-156.
    _Abstract_: Fueled by the rapid development of neuroscientific tools and techniques, some scholars consider the shift from traditional cognitive psychology toward cognitive neuroscience to be a _revolution_ (most notably Boone and Piccinini). However, the term “revolution” in philosophy of science can easily be construed as involving a paradigm shift in the sense of Kuhn’s _The Structure of Scientific Revolutions_. Is a Kuhnian account sound in the case at hand? To answer this question, we consider heuristic indicators of two (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31. Cognitive Primitives of Collective Intentions: Linguistic Evidence of Our Mental Ontology.Natalie Gold & Daniel Harbour - 2012 - Mind and Language 27 (2):109-134.
    Theories of collective intentions must distinguish genuinely collective intentions from coincidentally harmonized ones. Two apparently equally apt ways of doing so are the ‘neo-reductionism’ of Bacharach (2006) and Gold and Sugden (2007a) and the ‘non-reductionism’ of Searle (1990, 1995). Here, we present findings from theoretical linguistics that show that we is not a cognitive primitive, but is composed of notions of I and grouphood. The ramifications of this finding on the structure both of grammatical and lexical systems suggests that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework.Stanislas Dehaene & Lionel Naccache - 2001 - Cognition 79 (1):1-37.
    This introductory chapter attempts to clarify the philosophical, empirical, and theoretical bases on which a cognitive neuroscience approach to consciousness can be founded. We isolate three major empirical observations that any theory of consciousness should incorporate, namely (1) a considerable amount of processing is possible without consciousness, (2) attention is a prerequisite of consciousness, and (3) consciousness is required for some specific cognitive tasks, including those that require durable information maintenance, novel combinations of operations, or the spontaneous generation (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   434 citations  
  33.  13
    Cognitive diversity and the contingency of evidence.Jack C. Lyons - 2022 - Synthese 200 (3):1-20.
    Many epistemologists endorse a view I call “evidence essentialism:” if e is evidence of h, for some agent at some time, then necessarily, e is evidence of h, for any agent at any time. I argue that such a view is only plausible if we ignore cognitive diversity among epistemic agents, i.e., the fact that different agents have different—sometimes radically different—cognitive skills, abilities, and proclivities. Instead, cognitive diversity shows that evidential relations are contingent and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  2
    Cognition, Epistemology, and Reasoning about Evidence within the Legal Domain.Enrique Cáceres - 2008 - Problema. Anuario de Filosofía y Teoria Del Derecho 1 (2):243-264.
    Taking as a point of departure Maturana’s model of cognition, and combining it with his own general approach to the law, which he calls “Legal Constructivism”, the author analyzes the complex dynamics of judicial proof in terms of explaining how judges cognitively process the evidence put forward by the parties during a trial.The author advances the view that judges, just as scientists do, belong to a certain cognitive community, that is, to a certain judicial cognitive community. The (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  3
    No evidence for a common self-bias across cognitive domains.Annabel D. Nijhof, Kimron L. Shapiro, Caroline Catmur & Geoffrey Bird - 2020 - Cognition 197 (C):104186.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  36.  33
    Teaching Cognitive Skill through Dance: Evidence for near but Not Far Transfer.Mia Keinänen, Lois Hetland & Ellen Winner - 2000 - The Journal of Aesthetic Education 34 (3/4):295.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  37.  24
    Social cognition and learning mechanisms: Experimental evidence in domestic chicks.Jonathan N. Daisley, Orsola Rosa Salva, Lucia Regolin & Giorgio Vallortigara - 2011 - Interaction Studies 12 (2):208-232.
    In this paper we review the literature on social learning mechanisms in the domestic chick, focusing largely on work from our own laboratories. The domestic chicken is a social-living bird that searches for food in flocks, avoids predators by following warnings from other flock members, and forms (stable) social hierarchies. All of these behaviors develop throughout ontogeny, largely during the very early stages post-hatch. Newly hatched chicks appear to have predispositions to orient towards and to pay greatest attention to the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  56
    Cognitive appraisals and emotional experience: Further evidence.A. S. R. Manstead, Philip E. Tetlock & Tony Manstead - 1989 - Cognition and Emotion 3 (3):225-239.
  39.  11
    Cognitive Primitives of Collective Intentions: Linguistic Evidence of Our Mental Ontology.Daniel Harbour Natalie Gold - 2012 - Mind and Language 27 (2):109-134.
    Theories of collective intentions must distinguish genuinely collective intentions from coincidentally harmonized ones. Two apparently equally apt ways of doing so are the ‘neo‐reductionism’ of Bacharach (2006) and Gold and Sugden (2007a) and the ‘non‐reductionism’ of Searle (1990, 1995). Here, we present findings from theoretical linguistics that show that we is not a cognitive primitive, but is composed of notions of I and grouphood. The ramifications of this finding on the structure both of grammatical and lexical systems suggests that (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  40.  12
    Cognitive foundations of topic-comment and foreground-background structures: Evidence from sign languages, cospeech gesture and homesign.Elisabeth Engberg-Pedersen - 2011 - Cognitive Linguistics 22 (4):691-718.
    Krifka (The origin of topic/comment structure, of predication, and of focusation in asymmetric bimanual coordination, 2006, Interdisciplinary Studies on Information Structure (ISIS): Working Papers of SFB 632 08: 61–96, 2007b) suggests that asymmetric bimanual coordination and ultimately the evolution of lateralization in humans may be the cognitive basis of linguistic topic-comment structure and foreground-background structures in general. As asymmetric bimanual constructions abound in sign languages and are also found in their possible precursors, cospeech gesture and homesign, sign languages may (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  11
    Cognitive Reappraisal in Children: Neuropsychological Evidence of Up-Regulating Positive Emotion From an ERP Study.Wen Liu, Fang Liu, Liang Chen, Zhongqing Jiang & Junchen Shang - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  42.  10
    Cognitive aspects of participation: Evidence from two studies.Andrzej Bielecki & Ryszard Stocki - 2012 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 43 (4):310-323.
    The trouble of proving the effects of participation lies in the mismatch between three aspects of ownership: physical, legal and psychological. In our interdisciplinary systemic model of ownership, we propose 10 relationships related to ownership/participation from: „A is a part of B” - greatest involvement to „A does not know about B” - the least involvement. „A” and „B” may take different values of: a person, an institution, a community, a group, an object. Once formalized we can view the studies (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  11
    No Evidence of Narrowly Defined Cognitive Penetrability in Unambiguous Vision.Nikki A. Lammers, Edward H. de Haan & Yair Pinto - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  44.  15
    Cognition can affect perception: Restating the evidence of a top-down effect.Daniel T. Levin, Lewis J. Baker & Mahzarin R. Banaji - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  45.  3
    Cognitive Mechanisms of Monolingual and Bilingual Children in Monoliterate Educational Settings: Evidence From Sentence Repetition.Maria Andreou, Ianthi Maria Tsimpli, Elvira Masoura & Eleni Agathopoulou - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Sentence repetition tasks have been extensively employed to assess bilingual children’s linguistic and cognitive resources. The present study examined whether monoliterate bilingual children differ from their monolingual peers in SR accuracy and cognitive tasks, and investigated links between vocabulary, updating, verbal and visuospatial working memory and SR performance in the same children. Participants were two groups of 35 children, 8–12 years of age: one group consisted of Albanian-Greek monoliterate bilingual children and the other of Greek monolingual children attending (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  36
    Does evidence from ethology support bicoded cognitive maps?Shane Zappettini & Colin Allen - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):570-571.
    The presumption that navigation requires a cognitive map leads to its conception as an abstract computational problem. Instead of loading the question in favor of an inquiry into the metric structure and evolutionary origin of cognitive maps, the task should first be to establish that a map-like representation actually is operative in real animals navigating real environments.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47.  16
    Computational Evidence for the Subitizing Phenomenon as an Emergent Property of the Human Cognitive Architecture.Scott A. Peterson & Tony J. Simon - 2000 - Cognitive Science 24 (1):93-122.
    A computational modeling approach was used to test one possible explanation for the limited capacity of the subitizing phenomenon. Most existing models of this phenomenon associate the subitizing span with an assumed structural limitation of the human information processing system. In contrast, we show how this limit might emerge as the combinatorics of the space of enumeration problems interacts with the human cognitive architecture in the context of an enumeration task. Subitizing‐like behavior was generated in two different models of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  48.  51
    Language, cognition, and the nature of modularity: Evidence from aphasia.Rosemary Varley & Michael Siegal - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (6):702-703.
    We examine Carruthers’ proposal that sentences in logical form serve to create flexibility within central system modularity, enabling the combination of information from different modalities. We discuss evidence from aphasia and the neurobiology of input-output systems. This work suggests that there exists considerable capacity for interdomain cognitive processing without language mediation. Other challenges for a logical form account are noted.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49. On the relationship between cognitive models and spiritual maps. Evidence from Hebrew language mysticism.Brian L. Lancaster - 2000 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 7 (11-12):11-12.
    It is suggested that the impetus to generate models is probably the most fundamental point of connection between mysticism and psychology. In their concern with the relation between ‘unseen’ realms and the ‘seen’, mystical maps parallel cognitive models of the relation between ‘unconscious’ and ‘conscious’ processes. The map or model constitutes an explanation employing terms current within the respective canon. The case of language mysticism is examined to illustrate the premise that cognitive models may benefit from an understanding (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  50.  22
    Social cognition and learning mechanisms: Experimental evidence in domestic chicks.Jonathan N. Daisley, Orsola Rosa Salva, Lucia Regolin & Giorgio Vallortigara - 2011 - Interaction Studies 12 (2):208-232.
1 — 50 / 1000