Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Modularity need not imply locality: Damaged modules can have nonlocal effects.Edgar Zurif & David Swinney - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):89-90.
  • Number Conceptualisation among Lebanese Micro-Business Owners who Engage in Orally-Based Versus Paper-Based Numeracy Practices: An Experimental Cognitive Ethnography.Samar Zebian - 2008 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 8 (3-4):359-385.
    The study of everyday numeric thinking in adults directs our attention to several aspects of number cognition that have received almost no attention in the experimental cognitive science literature, namely the influences of socially situated artifact use on numeric processing. The current studies explore numeral recognition and conceptualisation processes in business people who engage in different types of numeracy practices; orally based numeracy practices which involve very little use of written records compared to paper-based numeracy practices. Ethnographic observations of Lebanese (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What counts as local?Andrew W. Young - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):88-89.
  • The localization/distribution distinction in neuropsychology is related to the isomorphism/multiple meaning distinction in cell electrophysiology.Gerald S. Wasserman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):87-88.
  • The symbolic brain or the invisible hand?René van Hezewijk & Edward H. F. de Haan - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):85-86.
  • The relation between language and arithmetic in bilinguals: insights from different stages of language acquisition.Amandine Van Rinsveld, Martin Brunner, Karin Landerl, Christine Schiltz & Sonja Ugen - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Playing Flourens to Fodor's Gall.Tim van Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-84.
  • Prosopagnosia, conscious awareness and the interactive brain.Robert Van Gulick - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-85.
  • Numerical representations: abstract or supramodal? Some may be spatial.Giuseppe Vallar & Luisa Girelli - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):354-355.
    The target article undermines the existence of a shared unitary numerical format, illustrating a variety of representations. The / dichotomy does not capture their specific features. These representations are with respect to the sensory modality of the stimulus, and independent of its specific notation, with a main role of spatial codes, both related and unrelated to the mental number line.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The functional architecture of visual attention may still be modular.Carlo Umiltà - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):82-83.
  • Language and number: a bilingual training study.Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2001 - Cognition 78 (1):45-88.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • The real functional architecture is gray, wet and slippery.Steven L. Small - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):81-82.
  • Throwing out the neuropsychological data with the locality bathwater?Philip Servos & Elizabeth M. Olds - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):80-81.
  • Locus-pocus.Carlo Semenza - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):80-80.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perception and its interactive substrate: Psychophysical linking hypotheses and psychophysical methods.Robert Sekuler - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):79-79.
  • Spatial complexity of character-based writing systems and arithmetic in primary school: a longitudinal study.Maja Rodic, Tatiana Tikhomirova, Tatiana Kolienko, Sergey Malykh, Olga Bogdanova, Dina Y. Zueva, Elena I. Gynku, Sirui Wan, Xinlin Zhou & Yulia Kovas - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line.Bert Reynvoet & Marc Brysbaert - 1999 - Cognition 72 (2):191-201.
    Many theories about human number representation stress the importance of a central semantic representation that includes the magnitude information of small integer numbers, and that is conceived as an abstract, compressed number line. However, thus far there has been little or no direct evidence that units and teens are represented on the same number line. In two masked priming experiments, we show that single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals are equally well primed by an Arabic numeral with the same number of (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Number word structure in first and second language influences arithmetic skills.Anat Prior, Michal Katz, Islam Mahajna & Orly Rubinsten - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Local and distributed processes in attentional orienting.Michael I. Posner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):78-79.
  • Parallel distributed processing challenges the strong modularity hypothesis, not the locality assumption.David C. Plaut - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):77-78.
  • Modeling the left digit effect in adult number line estimation.Andrea L. Patalano, Kelsey Kayton & Hilary Barth - 2023 - Cognition 230 (C):105257.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Symbolic magnitude modulates perceptual strength in binocular rivalry.Chris L. E. Paffen, Sarah Plukaard & Ryota Kanai - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):468-475.
  • Computational levels again.Mike Oaksford - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):76-77.
  • Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins.Hans-Christoph Nuerk, Ulrich Weger & Klaus Willmes - 2001 - Cognition 82 (1):B25-B33.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  • About the influence of the presentation format on arithmetical-fact retrieval processes.Marie-Pascale Noël, Wim Fias & Marc Brysbaert - 1997 - Cognition 63 (3):335-374.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Tracking Familial History of Reading and Math Difficulties in Children’s Academic Outcomes.Tin Q. Nguyen, Amanda Martinez-Lincoln & Laurie E. Cutting - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    The current study aimed to investigate the extent to which familial history of reading and math difficulties have an impact on children’s academic outcomes within a 3-year longitudinal study, which evaluated their core reading and math skills after first and second grades, as well as performance on complex academic tasks after second and third grades. At baseline, parents were asked to complete the Adult Reading History Questionnaire and its adaption, Adult Math History Questionnaire, to index familial history of reading and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Visuospatial and mathematical dysfunction in major depressive disorder and/or panic disorder: A study of parietal functioning.Brady D. Nelson & Stewart A. Shankman - 2016 - Cognition and Emotion 30 (3):417-429.
  • Why are you talking to yourself? The epistemic role of inner speech in reasoning.Wade Munroe - 2022 - Noûs 56 (4):841-866.
    People frequently report that, at times, their thought has a vocal character. Thinking commonly appears to be accompanied or constituted by silently ‘talking’ to oneself in inner speech. In this paper, we explore the specifically epistemic role of inner speech in conscious reasoning. A plausible position—but one I argue is ultimately wrong—is that inner speech plays asolelyfacilitative role that is exhausted by (i) serving as the vehicle of representation for conscious reasoning, and/or (ii) allowing one to focus on certain types (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A Unitary or Multiple Representations of Numerical Magnitude? – the Case of Structure in Symbolic and Non-Symbolic Quantities.Korbinian Moeller, Elise Klein, Hans-Christoph Nuerk & Roi Cohen Kadosh - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Distributed locality and large-scale neurocognitive networks.M. Marsel Mesulam - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):74-76.
  • Neuropsychology: Going loco?Rosaleen A. McCarthy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):73-74.
  • Arithmetic operation and working memory: differential suppression in dual tasks.Kyoung-Min Lee & So-Young Kang - 2002 - Cognition 83 (3):B63-B68.
  • Categories of Large Numbers in Line Estimation.David Landy, Arthur Charlesworth & Erin Ottmar - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (2):326-353.
    How do people stretch their understanding of magnitude from the experiential range to the very large quantities and ranges important in science, geopolitics, and mathematics? This paper empirically evaluates how and whether people make use of numerical categories when estimating relative magnitudes of numbers across many orders of magnitude. We hypothesize that people use scale words—thousand, million, billion—to carve the large number line into categories, stretching linear responses across items within each category. If so, discontinuities in position and response time (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Do neuropsychologists think in terms of interactive models?Marcel Kinsbourne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):72-73.
  • Sixty-four or four-and-sixty? The influence of language and working memory on children's number transcoding.Ineke Imbo, Charlotte Vanden Bulcke, Jolien De Brauwer & Wim Fias - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
  • Go with the flow but mind the details.Glyn W. Humphreys & M. Jane Riddoch - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):71-72.
  • Conceptual Integration of Arithmetic Operations With Real‐World Knowledge: Evidence From Event‐Related Potentials.Amy M. Guthormsen, Kristie J. Fisher, Miriam Bassok, Lee Osterhout, Melissa DeWolf & Keith J. Holyoak - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (3):723-757.
    Research on language processing has shown that the disruption of conceptual integration gives rise to specific patterns of event-related brain potentials —N400 and P600 effects. Here, we report similar ERP effects when adults performed cross-domain conceptual integration of analogous semantic and mathematical relations. In a problem-solving task, when participants generated labeled answers to semantically aligned and misaligned arithmetic problems, the second object label in misaligned problems yielded an N400 effect for addition problems. In a verification task, when participants judged arithmetically (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • No threat to modularity.Yosef Grodzinsky & Uri Hadar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):70-71.
  • Clarifying the locality assumption.Clark Glymour - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):69-70.
  • Brain neural activity patterns yielding numbers are operators, not representations.Walter J. Freeman & Robert Kozma - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):336.
  • A Single-Boundary Accumulator Model of Response Times in an Addition Verification Task.Thomas J. Faulkenberry - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuropsychological inference with an interactive brain: A critique of the “locality” assumption.Martha J. Farah - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):43-61.
    When cognitive neuropsychologists make inferences about the functional architecture of the normal mind from selective cognitive impairments they generally assume that the effects of brain damage are local, that is, that the nondamaged components of the architecture continue to function as they did before the damage. This assumption follows from the view that the components of the functional architecture are modular, in the sense of being informationally encapsulated. In this target article it is argued that this “locality” assumption is probably (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   122 citations  
  • Interactions on the interactive brain.Martha J. Farah - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):90-104.
    When cognitive neuropsychologists make inferences about the functional architecture of the normal mind from selective cognitive impairments they generally assume that the effects of brain damage are local, that is, that the nondamaged components of the architecture continue to function as they did before the damage. This assumption follows from the view that the components of the functional architecture are modular, in the sense of being informationally encapsulated. In this target article it is argued that this “locality” assumption is probably (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Concrete magnitudes: From numbers to time.Christine Falter, Valdas Noreika, Julian Kiverstein & Bruno Mölder - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):335-336.
    Cohen Kadosh & Walsh (CK&W) present convincing evidence indicating the existence of notation-specific numerical representations in parietal cortex. We suggest that the same conclusions can be drawn for a particular type of numerical representation: the representation of time. Notation-dependent representations need not be limited to number but may also be extended to other magnitude-related contents processed in parietal cortex (Walsh 2003).
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Syntactic priming reveals an explicit syntactic representation of multi-digit verbal numbers.Dror Dotan, Ilya Breslavskiy, Haneen Copty-Diab & Vivian Yousefi - 2021 - Cognition 215 (C):104821.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Parallel and serial processes in number-to-quantity conversion.Dror Dotan & Stanislas Dehaene - 2020 - Cognition 204 (C):104387.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Neurocomputing and modularity.Joachim Diederich - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):68-69.
  • Further advantages of abandoning the locality assumption in face recognition.Jules Davidoff & Bernard Renault - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):68-68.
  • Numerical representation in the parietal lobes: Abstract or not abstract?Roi Cohen Kadosh & Vincent Walsh - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):313-328.
    The study of neuronal specialisation in different cognitive and perceptual domains is important for our understanding of the human brain, its typical and atypical development, and the evolutionary precursors of cognition. Central to this understanding is the issue of numerical representation, and the question of whether numbers are represented in an abstract fashion. Here we discuss and challenge the claim that numerical representation is abstract. We discuss the principles of cortical organisation with special reference to number and also discuss methodological (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Modularity, abstractness and the interactive brain.James M. Clark - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):67-68.