Results for 'Vervet-monkey'

670 found
Order:
  1.  9
    Corrigendum: Dynamics of Intersexual Dominance and Adult Sex- Ratio in Wild Vervet Monkeys.Charlotte Korinna Hemelrijk, Matthias Wubs, Gerrit Gort, Jennifer Botting & Erica van de Waal - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  12
    Dynamics of Intersexual Dominance and Adult Sex- Ratio in Wild Vervet Monkeys.Charlotte Korinna Hemelrijk, Matthias Wubs, Gerrit Gort, Jennifer Botting & Erica van de Waal - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3. Intergroup Variation of Social Relationships in Wild Vervet Monkeys: A Dynamic Network Approach.Christèle Borgeaud, Sebastian Sosa, Redouan Bshary, Cédric Sueur & Erica van de Waal - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  27
    What’s in a name? The vervet predator calls and the limits of the Washburnian synthesis.Gregory Radick - 2006 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 37 (2):334-362.
    After the Second World War, a renaissance in field primatology took place in the United States under the aegis of the ‘new physical anthropology’. Its leader, Sherwood Washburn, envisioned a science uniting studies of hominid fossils with Darwinian population genetics, experimental functional anatomy, and field observation of non-human primates and human hunter–gatherers. Thanks to Washburn’s stimulus, his colleague at Berkeley, the bird ethologist Peter Marler, took up the study of the natural communicative behaviour of apes and monkeys. When Marler’s first (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  5.  46
    Dicent Symbols in Non-Human Semiotic Processes.João Queiroz - 2012 - Biosemiotics 5 (3):319-329.
    Against the view that symbol-based semiosis is a human cognitive uniqueness, we have argued that non-human primates such as African vervet monkeys possess symbolic competence, as formally defined by Charles S. Peirce. Here I develop this argument by showing that the equivocal role ascribed to symbols by “folk semiotics” stems from an incomplete application of the Peircean logical framework for the classification of signs, which describes three kinds of symbols: rheme, dicent and argument. In an attempt to advance in (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  6. Goanna ranch, captive bred, spcializing in rare monitors both dwarf and large, blackheaded and woma pythons, for list send sase to goanna ranch, po box 85036, tucson.Chacoan Monkey Frogs - 1998 - Vivarium 9:65.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7. Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The 'panglossian paradigm' defended.Daniel C. Dennett - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):343-90.
    Ethologists and others studying animal behavior in a spirit are in need of a descriptive language and method that are neither anachronistically bound by behaviorist scruples nor prematurely committed to particular Just such an interim descriptive method can be found in intentional system theory. The use of intentional system theory is illustrated with the case of the apparently communicative behavior of vervet monkeys. A way of using the theory to generate data - including usable, testable data - is sketched. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   595 citations  
  8. Communication before communicative intentions.Josh Armstrong - 2021 - Noûs 57 (1):26-50.
    This paper explores the significance of intelligent social behavior among non-human animals for philosophical theories of communication. Using the alarm call system of vervet monkeys as a case study, I argue that interpersonal communication (or what I call “minded communication”) can and does take place in the absence of the production and recognition of communicative intentions. More generally, I argue that evolutionary theory provides good reasons for maintaining that minded communication is both temporally and explanatorily prior to the use (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  9.  80
    Evolutionary Psychology, Ethology, and Essentialism (Because What They Don't Know Can Hurt Us).Letitia Meynell - 2012 - Hypatia 27 (1):3-27.
    In 2002, Evolution and Human Behavior published a study purporting to show that the differences in toy preferences commonly attributed to girls and boys can also be found in male and female vervet monkeys, tracing the origin of these differing preferences back to a common ancestor. Despite some flaws in its design and the prima facie implausibility of some of its central claims, this research received considerable attention in both scientific circles and the popular media. In what follows, I (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  10.  31
    Primate Language and the Playback Experiment, in 1890 and 1980.Gregory Radick - 2005 - Journal of the History of Biology 38 (3):461-493.
    The playback experiment -- the playing back of recorded animal sounds to the animals in order to observe their responses -- has twice become central to celebrated researches on non-human primates. First, in the years around 1890, Richard Garner, an amateur scientist and evolutionary enthusiast, used the new wax cylinder phonograph to record and reproduce monkey utterances with the aim of translating them. Second, in the years around 1980, the ethologists Peter Marler, Robert Seyfarth, and Dorothy Cheney used tape (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  11. The Importance of Being Erroneous.David Beisecker - 1999 - Philosophical Topics 27 (1):281-308.
    The question of animal belief (or animal intentionality) often degenerates into a frustrating and unproductive exchange. Foes of animal intentionality point out that non-linguistic animals couldn’t possibly possess the kinds of mental states we linguistic beings enjoy. They claim that linguistic ability enables us to become sensitive to intensional contexts or to the states of mind of others in a way that is unavailable to the non-linguistic, and that would be necessary for proper attributions of intentionality. To attribute mental states (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  12. Our understanding of other minds: theory of mind and the intentional stance.Kristin Andrews - 2000 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 7 (7):12-24.
    Psychologists distinguish between intentional systems which have beliefs and those which are also able to attribute beliefs to others. The ability to do the latter is called having a 'theory of mind', and many cognitive ethologists are hoping to find evidence for this ability in animal behaviour. I argue that Dennett's theory entails that any intentional system that interacts with another intentional system (such as vervet monkeys and chess-playing computers) has a theory of mind, which would make the distinction (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  13.  44
    Art and evolution: Spiegelman's the narrative corpse.Brian Boyd - 2008 - Philosophy and Literature 32 (1):pp. 31-57.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Art and Evolution:Spiegelman's The Narrative CorpseBrian BoydIHas art evolved, like opposable thumbs and the whites of our eyes? If it has, will knowing so help us understand better not just art in general but particular works, even works of avant-garde art? Over recent decades many have come to accept that not only have humans evolved from other animals but that many features of their minds and behavior can be (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  21
    On Quantitative Comparative Research in Communication and Language Evolution.D. Kimbrough Oller & Ulrike Griebel - 2014 - Biological Theory 9 (3):296-308.
    Quantitative comparison of human language and natural animal communication requires improved conceptualizations. We argue that an infrastructural approach to development and evolution incorporating an extended interpretation of the distinctions among illocution, perlocution, and meaning can help place the issues relevant to quantitative comparison in perspective. The approach can illuminate the controversy revolving around the notion of functional referentiality as applied to alarm calls, for example in the vervet monkey. We argue that referentiality offers a poor point of quantitative (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  15.  33
    Primate Social Intelligence.Robert P. Worden - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (4):579-616.
    A computational theory of primate social intelligence is proposed in which primates represent social situations internally by discrete symbol structures, called scripts. Three well‐defined computational operations on scripts are sufficient to support social learning, planning, and prediction. This gives a formal, predictive model with which to analyse how primate social knowledge is acquired, as well as how it is used.The theory is compared with primate data, such as Cheney and Seyfarth's observations of vervet monkeys. It gives simple, understandable script‐based (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  16.  10
    Conceptual Analysis and Empirical Observations of Animal Minds.Ricardo Parellada - 2021 - Philosophia 49 (5):2197-2210.
    The relation between conceptual analysis and empirical observations when ascribing or denying concepts and beliefs to non-human animals is not straightforward. In order to reflect on this relation, I focus on two theoretical proposals and one empirical case, the three of which are permanently discussed and considered in the literature on animal cognition. First, I review briefly Davidson’s arguments for denying thought to non-linguistic animals. Second, I review Allen’s criteria for ascribing concepts to creatures capable of correcting their discriminatory powers (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  27
    The Intentional Stance. [REVIEW]Edward N. Zalta - 1989 - Review of Metaphysics 43 (2):397-400.
    In this book, Dennett determines just how far we can push the idea that mental states are distinguished by intentionality, that is, by the fact that they have content in virtue of being about, or directed towards, the world at large. Intentionality is characteristic of such states as belief and desire, since all belief is belief of something or that something be the case. In contrast to the physical stance and the design stance, the intentional stance is the predictive attitude (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  96
    How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species.Dorothy L. Cheney & Robert M. Seyfarth - 1990 - University of Chicago Press.
    "This reviewer had to be restrained from stopping people in the street to urge them to read it: They would learn something of the way science is done,...
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   575 citations  
  19. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics.Michael A. Arbib - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (2):105-124.
    The article analyzes the neural and functional grounding of language skills as well as their emergence in hominid evolution, hypothesizing stages leading from abilities known to exist in monkeys and apes and presumed to exist in our hominid ancestors right through to modern spoken and signed languages. The starting point is the observation that both premotor area F5 in monkeys and Broca's area in humans contain a “mirror system” active for both execution and observation of manual actions, and that F5 (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   126 citations  
  20.  55
    Monkey semantics: two ‘dialects’ of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls.Philippe Schlenker, Emmanuel Chemla, Kate Arnold, Alban Lemasson, Karim Ouattara, Sumir Keenan, Claudia Stephan, Robin Ryder & Klaus Zuberbühler - 2014 - Linguistics and Philosophy 37 (6):439-501.
    We develop a formal semantic analysis of the alarm calls used by Campbell’s monkeys in the Tai forest and on Tiwai island —two sites that differ in the main predators that the monkeys are exposed to. Building on data discussed in Ouattara et al. :e7808, 2009a; PNAS 106: 22026–22031, 2009b and Arnold et al., we argue that on both sites alarm calls include the roots krak and hok, which can optionally be affixed with -oo, a kind of attenuating suffix; in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  21.  14
    Capuchin monkeys do not show human-like pricing effects.Rhia Catapano, Nicholas Buttrick, Jane Widness, Robin Goldstein & Laurie R. Santos - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:111567.
    Recent work in judgment and decision-making has shown that a good’s price can have irrational effects on people’s preferences. People tend to prefer goods that cost more money and assume that such expensive goods will be more effective, even in cases where the price of the good is itself arbitrary. Although much work has documented the existence of these pricing effects, unfortunately little work has addressed where these price effects come from in the first place. Here we use a comparative (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22. Burning monkey-puzzle: Native fire ecology and forest management in northern Patagonia. [REVIEW]David Aagesen - 2004 - Agriculture and Human Values 21 (2-3):233-242.
    This article outlines the ecological and ethnobotanical characteristics of the monkey-puzzle tree (Araucariaaraucana), a long-lived conifer of great importance to the indigenous population living in and around its range in the southern Andes. The article also considers the pre-Columbian and historical use of indigenous fire technology. Conclusive evidence of indigenous burning is unavailable. However, our knowledge of native fire ecology elsewhere and our understanding of monkey-puzzle's ecological response to fire suggest that indigenous people probably burned in the past (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  10
    Squirrel monkeys and discrimination learning: Figural interactions, redundancies, and random shapes.Allan J. Nash & Kenneth M. Michels - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (1):132.
  24.  22
    Monkey See, Monkey Do?”: The Effect of Construal Level on Consumers’ Reactions to Others’ Unethical Behavior.Yuanqiong He, Junfang Zhang, Yuanyuan Zhou & Zhilin Yang - 2019 - Journal of Business Ethics 156 (2):455-472.
    This research examines how and why reactions to other consumers’ unethical behavior differ among consumers and vary in different situations. Drawing on construal level theory, the authors propose that the relationship between other consumers’ unethical behavior and focal consumers’ unethical behavior is moderated by focal consumers’ construal level, and self-expressiveness mediates this moderating effect. Specifically, consumers at higher construal levels tend to view their behavior as more self-expressive and are thus less likely to imitate other consumers’ unethical behavior. Study 1 (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  25. Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers.Jonathan I. Flombaum, Justin A. Junge & Marc D. Hauser - 2005 - Cognition 97 (3):315-325.
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  26.  25
    Are monkeys nomothetic or idiographic?Linda Mealey - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):161-161.
  27.  27
    Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) map number onto space.Caroline B. Drucker & Elizabeth M. Brannon - 2014 - Cognition 132 (1):57-67.
  28.  28
    Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task.S. Gouteux, C. Thinus-Blanc & J. Vauclair - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (3):505.
  29.  61
    Blindsight in monkeys.Alan Cowey - 1995 - Nature 373:247-9.
  30. Finessing the Bored Monkey Problem.Ned Block - 2020 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 24 (1):1-2.
    This is a response to Ian Phillips and Jorge Morales, "The Fundamental Problem with No-Cognition Paradigms," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2020.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  31.  59
    Monkey mountain as a megazoo: Analyzing the naturalistic claims of" wild monkey parks" in Japan.John Knight - 2006 - Society and Animals 14 (3):245.
    In Japan, yaen kōen or "wild monkey parks" are popular visitor attractions that show free-ranging monkey troops to the paying public. Unlike zoos, which display nonhuman animals through confinement, monkey parks control the movements of the monkeys through provisioning. The parks project an image of themselves as "natural zoos," claiming to practice a more authentic form of displaying animals-in-the-wild than that practiced by the zoo. This article critically evaluates the monkey park's claim by examining park management (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  34
    Monkey Business: Imitation, Authenticity, and Identity from Pithekoussai to Plautus.Catherine Connors - 2004 - Classical Antiquity 23 (2):179-207.
    This essay explores references to monkeys as a way of talking about imitation, authenticity, and identity in Greek stories about the “Monkey Island” Pithekoussai and in Athenian insults, and in Plautus' comedy. In early Greek contexts, monkey business defines what it means to be aristocratic and authoritative. Classical Athenians use monkeys to think about what it means to be authentically Athenian: monkey business is a figure for behavior which threatens democratic culture—sycophancy or other deceptions of the people. (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  35
    Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans.Dustin J. Merritt, Daniel Casasanto & Elizabeth M. Brannon - 2010 - Cognition 117 (2):191-202.
  34.  13
    Macaque monkeys discriminate pitch relationships.Michael Brosch, Elena Selezneva, Cornelia Bucks & Henning Scheich - 2004 - Cognition 91 (3):259-272.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  35. Monkeys, typewriters, and objective consequentialism.Eric Wiland - 2005 - Ratio 18 (3):352–360.
    There have been several recent attempts to refute objective consequentialism on the grounds that it implies the absurd conclusion that even the best of us act wrongly. Some have argued that we act wrongly from time to time; others have argued that we act wrongly regularly. Here I seek to strengthen reductio arguments against objective consequentialism by showing that objective consequentialism implies that we almost never act rightly. I show that no matter what you do, there is almost certainly something (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  36.  26
    Monkeys match and tally quantities across senses.Kerry E. Jordan, Evan L. MacLean & Elizabeth M. Brannon - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):617-625.
  37.  21
    Using Monkeys to Understand and Cure Parkinson Disease.D. Eugene Redmond, Jr - 2012 - Hastings Center Report 42 (s1):7-11.
    Research with nonhuman primates is essential to medical progress and will still be necessary for the foreseeable future. Almost all research scientists agree that animal research is critical to understanding basic biology, discovering new treatments for human (and animal) diseases, and maximizing the safety of new medicines while minimizing their harm to humans. All but two of the Nobel prizes in medicine awarded over the last one hundred years have depended on animal research, and the list of modern medicines, vaccines, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  35
    Monkeys match and tally quantities across senses.Elizabeth M. Brannon Kerry E. Jordan, Evan L. MacLean - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):617.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  39.  18
    Capuchin monkeys judge third-party reciprocity.James R. Anderson, Ayaka Takimoto, Hika Kuroshima & Kazuo Fujita - 2013 - Cognition 127 (1):140-146.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  40.  16
    Three Monkeys. I Am Father.Will Baker - 1985 - Between the Species 1 (2):8.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  26
    Straw monkeys.Michael C. Corballis - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):269-270.
  42. Monkeying with Motives: Agent-Basing Virtue Ethics*: Julia Driver.Julia Driver - 1995 - Utilitas 7 (2):281-288.
    Virtue ethics has generated a great deal of excitement among ethicists largely because it is seen as an alternative to the traditional theories – utilitarianism and Kantian ethics – which have come under considerable scrutiny and criticism in the past 30 years. Rather than give up the enterprise of doing moral theory altogether, as some have suggested, others have opted to develop an alternative that would hopefully avoid the shortcomings of both utilitarianism and Kantian ethics. Several writers, such as Jorge (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  43.  25
    How monkeys do things with “words”.Simon Baron-Cohen - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):148-149.
  44.  23
    Monkeys are curious about counterfactual outcomes.Maya Zhe Wang & Benjamin Y. Hayden - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):1-10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  45.  35
    Do monkeys rank each other?Robert M. Seyfarth - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):447-448.
  46.  26
    Beyond monkey minds: Toward a richer cognitive ethology.Marc Bekoff, Susan E. Townsend & Dale Jamieson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):571-572.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  24
    Monkey see, monkey do: Learning relations through concrete examples.Marc T. Tomlinson & Bradley C. Love - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2):150-151.
    Penn et al. argue that the complexity of relational learning is beyond animals. We discuss a model that demonstrates relational learning need not involve complex processes. Novel stimuli are compared to previous experiences stored in memory. As learning shifts attention from featural to relational cues, the comparison process becomes more analogical in nature, successfully accounting for performance across species and development.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  43
    Monkeys mind.Colin Allen - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):147-147.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  24
    Monkeys and consciousness.D. M. Armstrong - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):147-148.
  50.  13
    The Monkey's Paw.Arthur L. Manure - 1972 - Journal of Social Philosophy 3 (1):1-4.
1 — 50 / 670