Results for 'Behavioral science'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  28
    Organizational Justice: A Behavioral Science Concept with Critical Implications for Business Ethics and Stakeholder Theory.LaRue Tone Hosmer & Christian Kiewitz - 2005 - Business Ethics Quarterly 15 (1):67-91.
    Abstract:Organizational justice is a behavioral science concept that refers to the perception of fairness of the past treatment of the employees within an organization held by the employees of that organization. These subjective perceptions of fairness have been empirically shown to be related to 1) attitudinal changes in job satisfaction, organizational commitment and managerial trust beliefs; 2) behavioral changes in task performance activities and ancillary extra-task efforts to assist group members and improve group methods; 3) numerical changes (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  2.  30
    Organizational Justice: A Behavioral Science Concept with Critical Implications for Business Ethics and Stakeholder Theory.Christian Kiewitz - 2005 - Business Ethics Quarterly 15 (1):67-91.
    Abstract:Organizational justice is a behavioral science concept that refers to the perception of fairness of the past treatment of the employees within an organization held by the employees of that organization. These subjective perceptions of fairness have been empirically shown to be related to 1) attitudinal changes in job satisfaction, organizational commitment and managerial trust beliefs; 2) behavioral changes in task performance activities and ancillary extra-task efforts to assist group members and improve group methods; 3) numerical changes (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  3.  65
    The behavioral sciences are historical sciences of emergent complexity.Larry Arnhart - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):18-19.
    Unlike physics and chemistry, the behavioral sciences are historical sciences that explain the fuzzy complexity of social life through historical narratives. Unifying the behavioral sciences through evolutionary game theory would require a nested hierarchy of three kinds of historical narratives: natural history, cultural history, and biographical history. (Published Online April 27 2007).
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4. Explanation in the Behavioral Sciences.Robert Borger & Frank Cioffi - 1973 - Synthese 26 (2):324-336.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  5.  10
    Ethics as a behavioral science.Archie J. Bahm - 1974 - Springfield, Ill.,: Thomas.
  6. Can the Behavioral Sciences Self-correct? A Social Epistemic Study.Felipe Romero - 2016 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 60 (C):55-69.
    Advocates of the self-corrective thesis argue that scientific method will refute false theories and find closer approximations to the truth in the long run. I discuss a contemporary interpretation of this thesis in terms of frequentist statistics in the context of the behavioral sciences. First, I identify experimental replications and systematic aggregation of evidence (meta-analysis) as the self-corrective mechanism. Then, I present a computer simulation study of scientific communities that implement this mechanism to argue that frequentist statistics may converge (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  7. Adaptationism, exaptationism, and evolutionary behavioral science.Paul W. Andrews, Steven W. Gangestad & Dan Matthews - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (4):534-547.
    In our target article, we discussed the standards of evidence that could be used to identify adaptations, and argued that building an empirical case that certain features of a trait are best explained by exaptation, spandrel, or constraint requires the consideration, testing, and rejection of adaptationist hypotheses. We are grateful to the 31 commentators for their thoughtful insights. They raised important issues, including the meaning of “exaptation”; whether Gould and Lewontin's critique of adaptationism was primarily epistemological or ontological; the necessity, (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  8. Explanation in the Behavioral Sciences: Confrontations.Robert Borger & Frank Cioffi - 1972 - Philosophy and Rhetoric 5 (4):248-255.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  41
    Responsible behavioral science generalizations and applications require much more than non-WEIRD samples.Vladimir J. Konečni - 2010 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2-3):98-99.
    There are many methodological considerations that adversely affect external validity as much as, or even more than, unrepresentative sampling does. Among suspect applications, especially worrisome is the incorporation of WEIRD-based findings regarding moral reasoning and retribution into normative expectations, such as might be held by international criminal tribunals in war-torn areas.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10. The behavioral sciences and the ends of education.William Gruen - 1959 - Philosophy of Education:101.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  15
    Good behavioral science has room for theology: Any room for God?Robert B. Glassman - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (6):737-738.
    This excellent outline of evolutionary hypotheses is compromised by severe reductionism. Other writings succeed in granting theism ontological significance without compromising rigor. The discussion of counterintuitiveness neglects coherence in memory. Bearing in mind our severely limited working memory capacity, susceptibility to religious mythologies may comprise an adaptive heuristic approach to summarizing the contingencies of the most far-reaching of life's problems.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  12.  36
    Towards uniting the behavioral sciences with a Gene-centered approach to altruism.R. Michael Brown & Stephanie L. Brown - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):19-20.
    We support the ambitious goal of unification within the behavioral sciences. We suggest that Darwinian evolution by means of natural selection can provide the integrative glue for this purpose, and we review our own work on selective investment theory (SIT), which is an example of how other-regarding preferences can be accommodated by a gene-centered account of evolution. (Published Online April 27 2007).
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  72
    Getting Real on Rationality—Behavioral Science, Nudging, and Public Policy.Andreas T. Schmidt - 2019 - Ethics 129 (4):511-543.
    The nudge approach seeks to improve people’s decisions through small changes in their choice environments. Nudge policies often work through psychological mechanisms that deviate from traditional notions of rationality. Because of that, some critics object that nudging treats people as irrational. Such treatment might be disrespectful in itself and might crowd out more empowering policies. I defend nudging against these objections. By defending a nonstandard, ecological model of rationality, I argue that nudging not only is compatible with rational agency but (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  14.  27
    Explanation in the Behavioral Sciences. [REVIEW]R. J. B. - 1971 - Review of Metaphysics 25 (1):141-141.
    This is an intelligently designed collection of essays dealing with a variety of key issues that are in the foreground of reflection on the social and behavioral sciences. The format followed is an ideal one: a key paper, a comment by a critic, and a reply. Thus, for example, Charles Taylor explains and defends teleological explanation of behavior and engages in an exchange with Robert Borger; and Noam Chomsky reviews the problems of explanation in linguistics and is challenged by (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  77
    Unifying the behavioral sciences II.Herbert Gintis - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):45-53.
    My response to commentators includes a suggestion that an additional principle be added to the list presented in the target article: the notion of human society as a complex adaptive system with emergent properties. In addition, I clear up several misunderstandings shared by several commentators, and explore some themes concerning future directions in the unification of the behavioral science. (Published Online April 27 2007).
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  16.  19
    Improving social and behavioral science by making replication mainstream: A response to commentaries.Rolf A. Zwaan, Alexander Etz, Richard E. Lucas & M. Brent Donnellan - 2018 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 41.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  17.  27
    Extending the behavioral sciences framework: Clarification of methods, predictions, and concepts.Alex Mesoudi & Kevin N. Laland - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):36-37.
    We applaud Gintis's attempt to provide an evolutionary-based framework for the behavioral sciences, and note a number of similarities with our own recent cultural evolutionary structure for the social sciences. Gintis's proposal would be further strengthened by a greater emphasis on additional methods to evolutionary game theory, clearer empirical predictions, and a broader consideration of cultural transmission. (Published Online April 27 2007).
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  11
    A Call for Behavioral Science in Embedded Bioethics.Kristin M. Kostick-Quenet, Benjamin Lang, Natalie Dorfman & J. S. Blumenthal-Barby - 2022 - Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 65 (4):672-679.
    ABSTRACT:Bioethicists today are taking a greater role in the design and implementation of emerging technologies by "embedding" within the development teams and providing their direct guidance and recommendations. Ideally, these collaborations allow ethical considerations to be addressed in an active, iterative, and ongoing process through regular exchanges between ethicists and members of the technological development team. This article discusses a challenge to this embedded ethics approach—namely, that bioethical guidance, even if embraced by the development team in theory, is not easily (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  23
    Diversity in the behavioral sciences.William Sacksteder - 1963 - Philosophy of Science 30 (4):375-395.
    It is fairly usual to argue or to assume that there is a single pattern of proper scientific endeavor for the behavioral sciences. This is based, one is inclined to say, on the superb model afforded by the natural science successes of the past few hundred years and on the common method which underlies and determines that model. Consequently, a protagonist can argue that within his own science, all opposed modes of thinking are archaic, peripheral, or unscientific, (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20. The Ethics of Influence: Government in the Age of Behavioral Science.Cass R. Sunstein (ed.) - 2016 - New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.
    In recent years, 'nudge units' or 'behavioral insights teams' have been created in the United States, the United Kingdom, Germany, and other nations. All over the world, public officials are using the behavioral sciences to protect the environment, promote employment and economic growth, reduce poverty, and increase national security. In this book, Cass R. Sunstein, the eminent legal scholar and best-selling co-author of Nudge, breaks new ground with a deep yet highly readable investigation into the ethical issues surrounding (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  21. A framework for the unification of the behavioral sciences.Herbert Gintis - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):1-16.
    The various behavioral disciplines model human behavior in distinct and incompatible ways. Yet, recent theoretical and empirical developments have created the conditions for rendering coherent the areas of overlap of the various behavioral disciplines. The analytical tools deployed in this task incorporate core principles from several behavioral disciplines. The proposed framework recognizes evolutionary theory, covering both genetic and cultural evolution, as the integrating principle of behavioral science. Moreover, if decision theory and game theory are broadened (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  22.  18
    Evolution & contextual behavioral science: an integrated framework for understanding, predicting, & influencing human behavior.David Sloan Wilson, Steven C. Hayes & Anthony Biglan (eds.) - 2018 - Oakland, Calif.: Context Press, an imprint of New Harbinger Publications.
    Evolutionary science (ES) and contextual behavioral science (CBS) have developed largely independently during the last half century. However, the earlier histories of these two bodies of knowledge are thoroughly entwined. ES provides a unifying theoretical framework for the biological sciences, and is increasingly being applied to human-related sciences. Meanwhile, CBS is concerned with influencing human behavior in a practical sense. This groundbreaking volume seeks to integrate ES and CBS to promote real, positive change in peoples' lives. Evolution (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23. Objectivity in the Human and Behavioral Sciences [Chapter 4 of Objectivity].Guy Axtell - 2016 - In Objectivity. Cambridge, UK; Malden, MA: Polity Press; Wiley. pp. 109-136.
    Contentious debate has played out in the ‘science wars’ generally, but perhaps nowhere has the possibility and value of objectivity been more controversial than in respect to the social sciences and historiography, the writing of history. Most of the individual social sciences took shape and became academic disciplines during the 19th century, and the issue of differences between studying humankind and studying the natural world goes back at least this far as well. How should we understand the relationship between (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  37
    Do the cognitive and behavioral sciences need each other?David W. Gow Jr - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):27-28.
    Game theory provides a descriptive or a normative account of an important class of decisions. Given the cognitive sciences' emphasis on explanation, unification with the behavioral sciences under a descriptive model would constitute a step backwards in their development. I argue for the interdependence of the cognitive and behavioral sciences and suggest that meaningful integration is already occurring through problem-based interdisciplinary research programs. (Published Online April 27 2007).
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  8
    The Role of Behavioral Science in Personalized Multimodal Prehabilitation in Cancer.Chloe Grimmett, Katherine Bradbury, Suzanne O. Dalton, Imogen Fecher-Jones, Meeke Hoedjes, Judit Varkonyi-Sepp & Camille E. Short - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Multimodal prehabilitation is increasingly recognized as an important component of the pre-operative pathway in oncology. It aims to optimize physical and psychological health through delivery of a series of tailored interventions including exercise, nutrition, and psychological support. At the core of this prescription is a need for considerable health behavior change, to ensure that patients are engaged with and adhere to these interventions and experience the associated benefits. To date the prehabilitation literature has focused on testing the efficacy of devised (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  6
    Law and behavioral sciences: why we need less purity rather than more.Peter Mascini - 2016 - The Hague, The Netherlands: Eleven International Publishing.
    In his inaugural lecture, Peter Mascini takes issue with the goal of scientific purity in the behavioral study of law conceived as the deliberate choice to postulate a limited number of universally applicable behavioral principles. The guiding principle of behavioral sociology is that law behaves in correspondence to social space, while the guiding principle of law and economics is that individuals behave rationally. Behavioral economics has challenged the principle of the rational actor and, consequently, has also (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27. The cognitive and behavioral sciences: Real patterns, real unity, real causes, but no supervenience.Don Ross & David Spurrett - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (5):637-647.
    Our response amplifies our case for scientific realism and the unity of science and clarifies our commitments to scientific unity, nonreductionism, behaviorism, and our rejection of talk of “emergence.” We acknowledge support from commentators for our view of physics and, responding to pressure and suggestions from commentators, deny the generality supervenience and explain what this involves. We close by reflecting on the relationship between philosophy and science.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28. Phenomena and Objects of Research in the Cognitive and Behavioral Sciences.Uljana Feest - 2017 - Philosophy of Science 84 (5):1165-1176.
    It is commonly held that research efforts in the cognitive and behavioral sciences are mainly directed toward providing explanations and that phenomena figure into scientific practice qua explananda. I contend that these assumptions convey a skewed picture of the research practices in question and of the role played by phenomena. I argue that experimental research often aims at exploring and describing “objects of research” and that phenomena can figure as components of, and as evidence for, such objects. I situate (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  29.  18
    New functionalism and the social and behavioral sciences.Lukas Beck & James D. Grayot - 2021 - European Journal for Philosophy of Science 11 (4):1-28.
    Functionalism about kinds is still the dominant style of thought in the special sciences, like economics, psychology, and biology. Generally construed, functionalism is the view that states or processes can be individuated based on what role they play rather than what they are constituted of or realized by. Recently, Weiskopf has posited a reformulation of functionalism on the model-based approach to explanation. We refer to this reformulation as ‘new functionalism’. In this paper, we seek to defend new functionalism and to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  30.  35
    Psychobiology, sex research and chimpanzees: philanthropic foundation support for the behavioral sciences at Yale University, 1923—41.Kersten Jacobson Biehn - 2008 - History of the Human Sciences 21 (2):21-43.
    Behavioral science research in American universities was promoted and influenced by philanthropic foundations. In the 1920s and 1930s, Rockefeller philanthropies in particular financed behavioral science research projects that promised to fulfill their mandates to `improve mankind', mandates that foundation officers transformed into an informal, loosely defined human engineering effort. Controlling behavior, especially sexual and social `dysfunction', was a major priority. The behavioral scientists at Yale University, led by president James R. Angell and `psychobiologist' Robert M. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  31.  20
    On confusing 'measure'with 'measurement'in the methodology of behavioral science.Mario Bunge - 1973 - In Mario Augusto Bunge (ed.), The Methodological Unity of Science. Boston: Reidel. pp. 105--122.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  32.  20
    Measuring the Intentional World: Realism, Naturalism, and Quantitative Methods in the Behavioral Sciences.J. D. Trout - 1998 - New York, US: OUP Usa.
    Scientific realism has been advanced as an interpretation of the natural sciences but never the behavioral sciences. This book introduces a novel version of scientific realism, Measured Realism, that characterizes the kind of theoretical progress in the social and psychological sciences that is uneven but indisputable. It proposes a theory of measurement, Population-Guided Estimation, that connects natural, psychological, and social scientific inquiry. Presenting quantitative methods in the behavioral sciences as at once successful and regulated by the world, the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  33. Logical positivism and the behavioral sciences.Michael Scriven - 1969 - In Peter Achinstein & Stephen Francis Barker (eds.), The Legacy of Logical Positivism: Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Baltimore,: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 195--209.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  34.  58
    An Evidence-Based Study of the Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences.Edouard Machery & Kara Cohen - 2012 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 63 (1):177-226.
    The disagreement between philosophers about the scientific worth of the evolutionary behavioral sciences (evolutionary psychology, human behavioral ecology, etc.) is in part due to the fact that critics and advocates of these sciences characterize them very differently. In this article, by analyzing quantitatively the citations made in the articles published in Evolution & Human Behavior between January 2000 and December 2002, we provide some evidence that undermines the characterization of the evolutionary behavioral sciences put forward by their (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  35.  5
    Economics and the Behavioral Sciences: a Desert Frontier?Kenneth E. Boulding - 1956 - Diogenes 4 (15):1-14.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  21
    Beyond playing 20 questions with nature: Integrative experiment design in the social and behavioral sciences.Abdullah Almaatouq, Thomas L. Griffiths, Jordan W. Suchow, Mark E. Whiting, James Evans & Duncan J. Watts - 2024 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 47:e33.
    The dominant paradigm of experiments in the social and behavioral sciences views an experiment as a test of a theory, where the theory is assumed to generalize beyond the experiment's specific conditions. According to this view, which Alan Newell once characterized as “playing twenty questions with nature,” theory is advanced one experiment at a time, and the integration of disparate findings is assumed to happen via the scientific publishing process. In this article, we argue that the process of integration (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37. Economics and the Behavioral Sciences: a Desert Frontier?Kenneth E. Boulding - 1956 - Diogenes 4 (15):1-14.
  38.  8
    Patrons of the Revolution: Ideals and Institutions in Postwar Behavioral Science.Hunter Crowther-Heyck - 2006 - Isis 97 (3):420-446.
    This essay argues that shifts in patronage for the postwar behavioral and social sciences were linked intimately to both intellectual and institutional changes. This broad argument comprises two subarguments: first, that there were in fact two distinct, successive patronage systems for postwar social science—not one, as is commonly assumed; and, second, that the first postwar patronage system played a major role in enabling a series of behavioral revolutions and interdisciplinary syntheses across the social sciences, while the second (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  39.  13
    Explanation in the Behavioral Sciences. [REVIEW]J. B. R. - 1971 - Review of Metaphysics 25 (1):141-141.
    This is an intelligently designed collection of essays dealing with a variety of key issues that are in the foreground of reflection on the social and behavioral sciences. The format followed is an ideal one: a key paper, a comment by a critic, and a reply. Thus, for example, Charles Taylor explains and defends teleological explanation of behavior and engages in an exchange with Robert Borger; and Noam Chomsky reviews the problems of explanation in linguistics and is challenged by (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40. Evolution and Contextual Behavioral Science.David Sloan Wilson & Steven C. Hayes - 2018 - In David Sloan Wilson, Steven C. Hayes & Anthony Biglan (eds.), Evolution & contextual behavioral science: an integrated framework for understanding, predicting, & influencing human behavior. Oakland, Calif.: Context Press, an imprint of New Harbinger Publications.
  41.  9
    The Measurement of Values, Behavioral Science and Philosophical Approaches.Rollo Handy - 1972 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 32 (4):573-574.
  42.  18
    Value theory and the behavioral sciences.Rollo Handy - 1969 - Springfield, Ill.,: Thomas.
  43.  7
    Value Theory and the Behavioral Sciences.R. F. Atkinson - 1971 - Philosophical Quarterly 21 (82):89-90.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  8
    Value Theory and the Behavioral Sciences.David H. Degrood - 1969 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 30 (2):303-304.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  21
    Confronting Variation in the Social and Behavioral Sciences.Stephen M. Downes - 2016 - Philosophy of Science 83 (5):909-920.
    I pose problems for the views that human nature should be the object of study in the social and behavioral sciences and that a concept of human nature is needed to guide research in these sciences. I proceed by outlining three research programs in the social sciences, each of which confronts aspects of human variation. Next, I present Elizabeth Cashdan and Grant Ramsey’s related characterizations of human nature. I go on to argue that the research methodologies they each draw (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  46.  88
    The place of ethics in a unified behavioral science.Peter Danielson - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):23-24.
    Behavioral science, unified in the way Gintis proposes, should affect ethics, which also finds itself in “disarray,” in three ways. First, it raises the standards. Second, it removes the easy targets of economic and sociobiological selfishness. Third, it provides methods, in particular the close coupling of theory and experiments, to construct a better ethics. (Published Online April 27 2007).
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences.G. Casella & R. Berger - 2001
  48. Symbolic Thought and Communication : 4. Symbolic Thought and Communication from a Contextual Behavioral Science Perspective.Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Yvonne Barnes-Holmes & Ciara McEnteggart - 2018 - In David Sloan Wilson, Steven C. Hayes & Anthony Biglan (eds.), Evolution & contextual behavioral science: an integrated framework for understanding, predicting, & influencing human behavior. Oakland, Calif.: Context Press, an imprint of New Harbinger Publications.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49. Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences.David Sloan Wilson & Elliott Sober - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):585-608.
    In both biology and the human sciences, social groups are sometimes treated as adaptive units whose organization cannot be reduced to individual interactions. This group-level view is opposed by a more individualistic one that treats social organization as a byproduct of self-interest. According to biologists, group-level adaptations can evolve only by a process of natural selection at the group level. Most biologists rejected group selection as an important evolutionary force during the 1960s and 1970s but a positive literature began to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   133 citations  
  50.  12
    Developing Cross-Cultural Data Infrastructures (CCDIs) for Research in Cognitive and Behavioral Sciences.Oskar Burger, Lydia Chen, Alejandro Erut, Frankie T. K. Fong, Bruce Rawlings & Cristine H. Legare - 2023 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 14 (2):565-585.
    Cross-cultural research provides invaluable information about the origins of and explanations for cognitive and behavioral diversity. Interest in cross-cultural research is growing, but the field continues to be dominated by WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) researchers conducting WEIRD science with WEIRD participants, using WEIRD protocols. To make progress toward improving cognitive and behavioral science, we argue that the field needs (1) data workflows and infrastructures to support long-term high-quality research that is compliant with open- (...) frameworks; (2) process and participation standards to ensure research is valid, equitable, participatory, and inclusive; (3) training opportunities and resources to ensure the highest standards of proficiency, ethics, and transparency in data collection and processing. Here we discuss infrastructures for cross-cultural research in cognitive and behavioral sciences which we call __Cross-Cultural Data Infrastructures (CCDIs).__ We recommend building global networks of psychologists, anthropologists, demographers, experimental philosophers, educators, and cognitive, learning, and data scientists to distill their procedural and methodological knowledge into a set of community standards. We identify key challenges including protocol validity, researcher diversity, community inclusion, and lack of detail in reporting quality assurance and quality control (QAQC) workflows. Our objective is to help promote dialogue and efforts towards consolidating robust solutions by working with a broad research community to improve the efficiency and quality of cross-cultural research. (shrink)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 1000