Results for ' visual contact'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  11
    Why We Mimic Emotions Even When No One is Watching: Limited Visual Contact and Emotional Mimicry.Michal Olszanowski & Monika Wróbel - 2024 - Emotion Review 16 (1):16-27.
    This article explores interpersonal functions of emotional mimicry under the absence versus the presence of visual contact between the interacting partners. We review relevant literature and stress that previous studies on the role of emotional mimicry were focused on imitative responses to facial displays. We also show that the rules explaining why people mimic facial expressions may be inapplicable when visual signals are unavailable (e.g., people attending an online meeting have their cameras off). Overall, our review suggests (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  10
    Synchronization in Singing Duo Performances: The Roles of Visual Contact and Leadership Instruction.Sara D'Amario, Helena Daffern & Freya Bailes - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  7
    The Visual Resource as a Response to the Precarity Related to Lack of Physical Contact During the Pandemic: A Symbolic Solution from Design Intelligence.Dora Ivonne Alvarez Tamayo - forthcoming - Semiotics:75-94.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  25
    The Effect of Eye Contact Is Contingent on Visual Awareness.Shan Xu, Shen Zhang & Haiyan Geng - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  62
    Varieties of visual perspectives.David J. Bennett - 2009 - Philosophical Psychology 22 (3):329-352.
    One often hears it said that our visual-perceptual contact with the world is “perspectival.” But this can mean quite different things. Three different senses in which our visual contact with the world is “perspectival” are distinguished. The first involves the detection or representation of behaviorally important relations, holding between a perceiving subject and the world. These include time to contact, body-scaled size, egocentric position, and direction of heading. The second perspective becomes at least explicitly manifest (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  6.  11
    Contact Heat Evoked Potentials in China: Normal Values and Reproducibility.Bo Sun, Hongfen Wang, Zhaohui Chen, Fang Cui, Fei Yang & Xusheng Huang - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Background: Contact heat evoked potentials is used to diagnose small fiber neuropathy. We established the normal values of CHEPs parameters in Chinese adults, optimized the test technique, and determined its reproducibility.Methods: We recruited 151 healthy adults. CHEPs was performed on the right forearm to determine the optimal number of stimuli, and then conducted at different sites to establish normal values, determine the effects of demographic characteristics and baseline temperature, and assess the short- and long-term reproducibility. N2 latency/height varied with (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7. Visual representations in science.William Goodwin - 2009 - Philosophy of Science 76 (3):372-390.
    This paper evaluates a general argument for the conclusion that visual representations in science must play the role of truth bearers if they are to figure as legitimate contributors to scientific arguments and explanations. The argument is found to be unsound. An alternative approach to assessing the role of visual representations in science is exemplified by an examination of the role of structural formulas in organic chemistry. Structural formulas are found not to play the role of truth bearers; (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  8.  64
    Case report: Dancing in the dark: A critical single case study engaging a blind father in the rehabilitation journey of his visually impaired child.Livio Provenzi, Giada Pettenati, Antonella Luparia, Daria Paini, Giorgia Aprile, Federica Morelli, Eleonora Mascherpa, Luisa Vercellino, Serena Grumi & Sabrina Signorini - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    BackgroundFace-to-face visual contact is a key component of the early parent-child interaction, therefore a visual impairment condition of the parent or the child represents a risk factor for dyadic patterns' development.AimsThe study presents a critical single case of a blind father and a 18-month-old visually impaired child. The study aims to explore changes in the relational functioning of this dyad during an early family-centered intervention.Methods and proceduresTen parent-child sessions were videotaped and micro-analytically coded. Data were analyzed through (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  10
    Visual post-occupancy evaluation of a restorative garden using virtual reality photography: Restoration, emotions, and behavior in older and younger people.Marco Boffi, Linda Grazia Pola, Elisabetta Fermani, Giulio Senes, Paolo Inghilleri, Barbara Ester Adele Piga, Gabriele Stancato & Natalia Fumagalli - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Natural environments have a restorative effect from mental/attentional fatigue, prevent stress, and help to revitalize psychological and physical resources. These benefits are crucial for promoting active aging, which is particularly relevant given the phenomenon of population aging in recent decades. To be considered restorative, green spaces have to meet specific requirements in ecological and psychological terms that can be assessed through Post-Occupancy Evaluation, a multimethod approach commonly used by environmental psychologists and landscape architects after construction to evaluate the design outcomes (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  10
    Visual Perception of Moisture Is a Pathogen Detection Mechanism of the Behavioral Immune System.Kazunori Iwasa, Takanori Komatsu, Ayaka Kitamura & Yuta Sakamoto - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    The behavioral immune system (BIS) includes perceptual mechanisms for detecting cues of contamination. Former studies have indicated that moisture has a disgusting property. Therefore, moisture could be a target for detecting contamination cues by the BIS. We conducted two experiments to examine the psychophysical basis of moisture perception and clarify the relationship between the perception of moisture and the BIS. We assumed that the number of high luminance areas in a visual image provided optical information that would enable the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  26
    Visual Geometry of Classical Japanese Gardens.Gert Jakobus van Tonder - 2022 - Axiomathes 32 (5):841-868.
    The concept of geometry may evoke a world of pure platonic shapes, such as spheres and cubes, but a deeper understanding of visual experience demands insight into the perceptual organization of naturalistic form. Japanese gardens excel as designed environments where the complex fractal geometry of nature has been simplified to a structural core that retains the essential properties of the natural landscape, thereby presenting an ideal opportunity for investigating the geometry and perceptual significance of such naturalistic characteristics. Here, fronto-parallel (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. Affordance as a Method in Visual Cultural Studies. Based on Theory and Tools of Vitality Semiotics.Martina Sauer - 2021 - Art Style International 2 (7):11-37.
    In a historiographical and methodological comparison of Formal Aesthetics and Iconology with the method of Affordance, the latter is to be introduced as a new method in Visual Cultural Studies. In extension ofepistemologically relevant aspects relatedtostyle and history of the artefacts, communicative and furthermoreaction and decisionrelevant aspects of artefacts become important. In this respect, it is the share of artefacts in life that the new method aims to uncover. The basis for this concern is the theory and methodological tools (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  13.  8
    Does Group Contact Shape Styles of Pictorial Representation? A Case Study of Australian Rock Art.C. Granito, J. J. Tehrani, J. R. Kendal & T. C. Scott-Phillips - 2022 - Human Nature 33 (3):237-260.
    Image-making is a nearly universal human behavior, yet the visual strategies and conventions to represent things in pictures vary greatly over time and space. In particular, pictorial styles can differ in their degree of figurativeness, varying from intersubjectively recognizable representations of things to very stylized and abstract forms. Are there any patterns to this variability, and what might its ecological causes be? Experimental studies have shown that demography and the structure of interaction of cultural groups can play a key (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  9
    Face-to-face contact during infancy: How the development of gaze to faces feeds into infants’ vocabulary outcomes.Zsofia Belteki, Carlijn van den Boomen & Caroline Junge - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Infants acquire their first words through interactions with social partners. In the first year of life, infants receive a high frequency of visual and auditory input from faces, making faces a potential strong social cue in facilitating word-to-world mappings. In this position paper, we review how and when infant gaze to faces is likely to support their subsequent vocabulary outcomes. We assess the relevance of infant gaze to faces selectively, in three domains: infant gaze to different features within a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  44
    From faces to hands: Changing visual input in the first two years.Caitlin M. Fausey, Swapnaa Jayaraman & Linda B. Smith - 2016 - Cognition 152 (C):101-107.
    Human development takes place in a social context. Two pervasive sources of social information are faces and hands. Here, we provide the first report of the visual frequency of faces and hands in the everyday scenes available to infants. These scenes were collected by having infants wear head cameras during unconstrained everyday activities. Our corpus of 143 hours of infant-perspective scenes, collected from 34 infants aged 1 month to 2 years, was sampled for analysis at 1/5 Hz. The major (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  16.  6
    Psychoanalytic Studies of Creativity, Greed and Fine Art: Making Contact with the Self.David P. Levine - 2015 - Routledge.
    Throughout the history of psychoanalysis, the study of creativity and fine art has been a special concern. _Psychoanalytic Studies of Creativity, Greed and Fine Art: Making Contact with the Self_ makes a distinct contribution to the psychoanalytic study of art by focusing attention on the relationship between creativity and greed. This book also focuses attention on factors in the personality that block creativity, and examines the matter of the self and its ability to be present and exist as the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  63
    Spatial limits on the nonvisual self-touch illusion and the visual rubber hand illusion: Subjective experience of the illusion and proprioceptive drift.Anne M. Aimola Davies, Rebekah C. White & Martin Davies - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (2):613-636.
    The nonvisual self-touch rubber hand paradigm elicits the compelling illusion that one is touching one’s own hand even though the two hands are not in contact. In four experiments, we investigated spatial limits of distance and alignment on the nonvisual self-touch illusion and the well-known visual rubber hand illusion. Common procedures and common assessment methods were used. Subjective experience of the illusion was assessed by agreement ratings for statements on a questionnaire and time of illusion onset. The nonvisual (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  18.  12
    Marx's Lost Aesthetic: Karl Marx and the Visual Arts.Margaret A. Rose - 1984 - Cambridge University Press.
    This book offers an original and challenging study of Marx's contact with the visual arts, aesthetic theories, and art policies in nineteenth-century Europe. It differs from previous discussions of Marxist aesthetic theory in looking at Marx's views from an art-historical rather than from a literary perspective, and in placing those views in the context of the art practices, theories, and policies of Marx's own time. Dr Rose begins her work by discussing Marx's planned treatise on Romantic art of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  19.  13
    Crossing the street in front of an autonomous vehicle: An investigation of eye contact between drivengers and vulnerable road users.Aïsha Sahaï, Elodie Labeye, Loïc Caroux & Céline Lemercier - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Communication between road users is a major key to coordinate movement and increase roadway safety. The aim of this work was to grasp how pedestrians, cyclists, and kick scooter users sought to visually communicate with drivengers when they would face autonomous vehicles. In each experiment, participants were asked to imagine themselves in described situations of encounters between a specific type of vulnerable road user and a human driver in an approaching car. The human driver state and the communicative means of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  8
    Two Types of Demonstration Through Guided Touch with Cane: Instruction Sequences in Orientation and Mobility Training for a Person with Visual Impairments.Yasusuke Minami, Hiro Yuki Nisisawa, Mitsuhiro Okada & Rui Sakaida - 2023 - Human Studies 46 (4):723-756.
    Persons with visual impairments (hereafter PVI) detect and discover obstacles and road conditions by touching with a white cane when walking on the streets. In one training session, an Orientation and Mobility specialist (hereafter SPT) guided a PVI by grasping and moving the cane that the PVI was holding. We conducted a multimodal analysis of two instruction sequences, one a "proving and achieving" demonstration (Sacks in Lectures on conversation, Blackwell, 1992) and the other a "learnable" (Zemel and Koschmann, in (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  25
    Children with autism and composite tactile-visual toys during parent-child interaction.Min-Yuan Ma & Ya-Hsueh Lee - 2014 - Interaction Studies 15 (2):260-291.
    Based on sensory integration theory, six fabric samples containing tactile and visual stimuli were selected using the sensory perceptions of designers and combined with balls. Experiments involving these toys were implemented with 15 families with preschool-aged high-functioning autistic children. The results showed that loose sequin , which possessed equal tactile and visual intensities, was strongly correlated with frequent smiling/laughing and high enjoyment levels. The fabric provided a loose tactile sensation regarding surface interweave uniformity and a bright visual (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  28
    Children with autism and composite tactile-visual toys during parent-child interaction.Min-Yuan Ma & Ya-Hsueh Lee - 2014 - Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies / Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies 15 (2):260-291.
    Based on sensory integration theory, six fabric samples containing tactile and visual stimuli were selected using the sensory perceptions of designers and combined with balls. Experiments involving these toys were implemented with 15 families with preschool-aged high-functioning autistic children. The results showed that loose sequin, which possessed equal tactile and visual intensities, was strongly correlated with frequent smiling/laughing and high enjoyment levels. The fabric provided a loose tactile sensation regarding surface interweave uniformity and a bright visual sensation (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  23.  11
    Applicability of a Novel Attunement Instrument and Its Relationship to Parental Sensitivity in Infants With and Without Visual Impairments.Victorita Stefania Vacaru, Andrea Urqueta Alfaro, Nadia Hoffman, Walter Wittich, Micky Stern, Heather J. Zar, Dan J. Stein & Paula Sophia Sterkenburg - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    This study investigated the applicability of a novel instrument to assess parent–child attunement in free play interactions, in dyads with an infant with and without visual impairments. We here report the findings on the reliability and applicability of the newly developed Attune & Stimulate Mother–Infant 56-items Instrument in two separate samples: one with infants with VI and one with typically sighted infants. In addition, we assessed the contribution of parental sensitivity to attunement in dyadic interactions. The A&S M-I is (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  81
    Malpighi, Swammerdam and the Colourful Silkworm: Replication and Visual Representation in Early Modern Science.Matthew Cobb - 2002 - Annals of Science 59 (2):111-147.
    In 1669, Malpighi published the first systematic dissection of an insect. The manuscript of this work contains a striking water-colour of the silkworm, which is described here for the first time. On repeating Malpighi's pioneering investigation, Swammerdam found what he thought were a number of errors, but was hampered by Malpighi's failure to explain his techniques. This may explain Swammerdam's subsequent description of his methods. In 1675, as he was about to abandon his scientific researches for a life of religious (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  25.  29
    Seeing Oneself in the Mirror: Critical Reflections on the Visual Experience of the Reflected Self.Frank Macke - 2005 - Journal of Phenomenological Psychology 36 (1):21-44.
    Merleau-Ponty, in his well-known essay, "Eye and Mind," startlingly comments: "A Cartesian does not see himself in the mirror; he sees a dummy, an 'outside,' which, he has every reason to believe, other people see in the very same way but which, no more for himself than for others, is not a body in the flesh." This essay opens up a discourse on this very problem: the question of what one sees when looks at oneself in the mirror. As well, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26. Setting things before the mind.Michael G. F. Martin - 1998 - In Anthony O'Hear (ed.), Current Issues in Philosophy of Mind. Oxford University Press. pp. 157--179.
    Listening to someone from some distance in a crowded room you may experience the following phenomenon: when looking at them speak, you may both hear and see where the source of the sounds is; but when your eyes are turned elsewhere, you may no longer be able to detect exactly where the voice must be coming from. With your eyes again fixed on the speaker, and the movement of her lips a clear sense of the source of the sound will (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  27. Setting Things before the Mind: M.G.F. Martin.M. G. F. Martin - 1998 - Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 43:157-179.
    Listening to someone from some distance in a crowded room you may experience the following phenomenon: when looking at them speak, you may both hear and see where the source of the sounds is; but when your eyes are turned elsewhere, you may no longer be able to detect exactly where the voice must be coming from. With your eyes again fixed on the speaker, and the movement of her lips a clear sense of the source of the sound will (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  28. Nature Screened: An Eco-Film-Phenomenology.Ilan Safit - 2014 - Environmental Philosophy 11 (2):211-235.
    Do cinematic representations of the natural world only put us in further remove from nature? A phenomenological approach shows that nature screened can produce a richer understanding of human–nature relations as these unfold in visual contact. If vision accesses the world in a unique relationship of sight, in which our contact with the world is defined by vision prior to any other interaction, the cinema offers a special setting for a phenomenology that seeks to draw-out the significance (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  29. Is seeing all it seems? Action, reason and the grand illusion.Andy Clark - 2002 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 9 (5-6):181-202.
    We seem, or so it seems to some theorists, to experience a rich stream of highly detailed information concerning an extensive part of our current visual surroundings. But this appearance, it has been suggested, is in some way illusory. Our brains do not command richly detailed internal models of the current scene. Our seeings, it seems, are not all that they seem. This, then, is the Grand Illusion. We think we see much more than we actually do. In this (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  30. Sullying Sights.Ryan P. Doran - 2022 - Philosophical Psychology 35 (2):177-204.
    In this article, an account of the architecture of the cognitive contamination system is offered, according to which the contamination system can generate contamination represen- tations in circumstances that do not satisfy the norms of contamination, including in cases of mere visual contact with disgusting objects. It is argued that this architecture is important for explaining the content, logic, distribution, and persistence of maternal impression beliefs – according to which fetal defects are caused by the pregnant mother’s experiences (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  31.  28
    Sign activity of mammals as means of ecological adaptation.Elina Vladimirova - 2009 - Sign Systems Studies 37 (3/4):614-635.
    The present article discusses different basic semiotic-scientific postulates regarding mammals’ sign activity. On the one hand, there are arguments denying animals sign activity, according to which mammals are not capable of semantic generalization on the basis of conventional linguistic values. According to another approach, mammals’ sign activity can be considered as means of ecological adaptation, that is, the features of animal behaviour based on the information, received by them through their habitat characteristics without direct visual contacts with their kind. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  32.  17
    Meaning Representationalism: between Representationalism and Qualia Realism.Roberto Horácio de Sá Pereira - 2016 - Grazer Philosophische Studien 93 (4):548-570.
    The purpose of this article is to offer a new view of the key relation between the content and the conscious character of visual experience. The author aims to support the following claims. First, the author rejects the qualia realist claim that conscious character is an intrinsic, nonrepresentational property of visual experience, for example, a pattern of activation of neurons. However, the author also rejects the rival widespread representationalist claim that the conscious character of visual experience is (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33. Proprioceptive Awareness and Practical Unity.Kathleen A. Howe - 2018 - Teorema: International Journal of Philosophy 37 (3):65-81.
    Deafferented subjects, while lacking proprioceptive awareness of much of their bodies, are nevertheless able to use their bodies in basic action. Sustained visual contact with the body parts of which they are no longer proprioceptively aware enables them to move these parts in a controlled way. This might be taken to straightforwardly show that proprioceptive awareness is inessential to bodily action. I, however, argue that this is not the case. Proprioceptive awareness figures essentially in our self-conscious unity as (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  34. Wizualizacje w poznaniu matematycznym a kategoria intuicji przestrzennej.Michał Sochański - 2013 - Filozofia Nauki 21 (1).
    After several decades during which diagrams were neglected as a reliable source of mathematical knowledge, in recent years we have witnessed a revival of interest in the role of diagrams in mathematical cognition. In the paper, I consider how those investigations relate to the concept of spatial intuition and its role in mathematical cognition. It is argued that some characteristics of mathematical cognition that involve the use of diagrams are analogous with characteristics usually attributed to intuitive knowledge, above all the (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35. Grand Illusion.Andy Clark - unknown
    We seem, or so it seems to some theorists, to experience a rich stream of highly detailed information concerning an extensive part of our current visual surroundings. But this appearance, it has been suggested, is in some way illusory. Our brains do not command richly detailed internal models of the current scene. Our seeings, it seems, are not all that they seem. This, then, is the Grand Illusion. We think we see much more than we actually do. In this (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36. Meaning Representationalism: Between Representationalism and Qualia Realism.da Sá Pereira Roberto Horácio - 2016 - Grazer Philosophische Studien 93.
    The purpose of this article is to offer a new view of the key relation between the content and the conscious character of visual experience. The author aims to support the following claims. First, the author rejects the qualia realist claim that conscious character is an intrinsic, nonrepresentational property of visual experience, for example, a pattern of activation of neurons. However, the author also rejects the rival widespread representationalist claim that the conscious character of visual experience is (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  2
    The mark of theory: inscriptive figures, poststructuralist prehistories.Andrea Bachner - 2018 - New York: Fordham University Press.
    "Cover"--"Title Page"--"Copyright" -- "Contents" -- "Introduction: At the Scene of Inscription" -- "1. Savage Marks: Subjection and the Specters of Anthropology" -- "2. Impact Erasure: Psychoanalysis and the Multiplication of Trauma" -- "3. Stings of Visibility: Picture Theories and Visual Contact" -- "4. Out of the Groove: Aural Traces and the Mediation of Sound" -- "Conclusion: Against Inscription?" -- "Acknowledgments" -- "Notes" -- "Works Cited.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  45
    Seeing through transparency: Performativity, vision and intent 1.Anne M. Cronin - 1994 - Cultural Values 3 (1):54-72.
    This paper engages with debates around transformations in the production and circulation of images and the changes in modes of perception that these offer. Paul Virilio has argued that technological developments have produced a shift in the site of meaning‐production from the material reference space of the image to the time of visual contact by the viewer. I consider what significance these temporalities have in relation to social difference, and I develop debates around the performative to consider how (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  8
    Staying in touch.Carolyn Korsmeyer - 2008 - In Garry Hagberg (ed.), Art and Ethical Criticism. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 185–210.
    This chapter contains sections titled: Three Examples Sameness of Experience Touch, Contact, Nearness, Presence Wright's Windows.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  40.  96
    Neurogeometry of v1 and Kanizsa contours.Jean Petitot - 2003 - Axiomathes 13 (3-4):347-363.
    We present a neuro-geometrical model for generating the shape of Kanizsa's modal subjective contours which is based on the functional architecture of the primary areas of the visual cortex. We focus on V1 and its pinwheel structure and model it as a discrete approximation of a continuous fibration π: R × P → P with base space the space of the retina R and fiber the projective line P of the orientations of the plane. The horizontal cortico-cortical connections of (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  41.  40
    A spatially oriented decision does not induce consciousness in a motor task.Bruce Bridgeman & Valerie Huemer - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):454-464.
    Visual information follows at least two branches in the human nervous system, following a common input stage: a cognitive ''what'' branch governs perception and experience, while a sensorimotor ''how'' branch handles visually guided behavior though its outputs are unconscious. The sensorimotor system is probed with an isomorphic task, requiring a 1:1 relationship between target position and motor response. The cognitive system, in contrast, is probed with a forced qualitative decision, expressed verbally, about the location of a target. Normally, the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  42.  95
    Transmural palliative care by means of teleconsultation: a window of opportunities and new restrictions. [REVIEW]Jelle van Gurp, Martine van Selm, Evert van Leeuwen & Jeroen Hasselaar - 2013 - BMC Medical Ethics 14 (1):12-.
    Background: Audio-visual teleconsultation is expected to help home-based palliative patients, hospital-based palliative care professionals, and family physicians to jointly design better, pro-active care. Consensual knowledge of the possibilities and limitations of teleconsultation in transmural palliative care is, however, largely lacking.This paper aims at describing elements of both the physical workplace and the cultural-social context of the palliative care practice, which are imperative for the use of teleconsultation technologies. Methods: A semi-structured expert meeting and qualitative, open interviews were deployed to (...)
    Direct download (20 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  38
    Inquiry, vision and objects: Foraging for coherence within neuroscience.Jay Schulkin - 2013 - Human Affairs 23 (4):616-632.
    We come prepared to track events and objects, building our knowledge base while foraging for coherence. Classical pragmatism recognizes that the acquisition of knowledge is in part a contact sport (e.g. Peirce, Dewey). One of the aims of neuroscience is to capture human experience. One route to perhaps achieve this may be through the study of the visual system and its expansion in our evolutionary history. Embodied cephalic systems, as Dewey knew well, are tied to self-corrective inquiry. A (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  43
    The safe haven of a new classicism: the quest for a new aesthetics in Hungary 1904–1912.Éva Forgács - 2008 - Studies in East European Thought 60 (1-2):75 - 95.
    Seen through the quest for a new metaphysics, the visual arts were interpreted in the framework of the particular sense of progress that the generation of György Lukács developed in the first decade of the twentieth century. They saw Impressionism as the veritable symptom of the deficiencies of their age and dreamed of a great, solid, lasting new Hungarian culture which would transcend the fragmentariness, sociological interests, and ethereality of Impressionism. Although exhibitions of contemporary modernist art were organized in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  45.  9
    #Filterdrop: Attending to Photographic Alterations.Claire Anscomb - 2023 - Nordic Journal of Aesthetics 32 (65-66).
    It is well-documented that the alteration of portrait photographs can have a negative impact on a viewer’s self-esteem. One might think that providing written disclaimers warning of alteration might help to mitigate this effect, yet empirical studies have shown that viewers continue to feel like what they are seeing is real, and thus attainable, despite knowing it is not. I propose that this cognitive dissonance occurs because disclaimers fail to show viewers how to look at the contents of a photographic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  46.  15
    Cognitive foundations of topic-comment and foreground-background structures: Evidence from sign languages, cospeech gesture and homesign.Elisabeth Engberg-Pedersen - 2011 - Cognitive Linguistics 22 (4):691-718.
    Krifka (The origin of topic/comment structure, of predication, and of focusation in asymmetric bimanual coordination, 2006, Interdisciplinary Studies on Information Structure (ISIS): Working Papers of SFB 632 08: 61–96, 2007b) suggests that asymmetric bimanual coordination and ultimately the evolution of lateralization in humans may be the cognitive basis of linguistic topic-comment structure and foreground-background structures in general. As asymmetric bimanual constructions abound in sign languages and are also found in their possible precursors, cospeech gesture and homesign, sign languages may serve (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  10
    Images Made by Contagion: On Dermatological Wax Moulages.Mechthild Fend - 2022 - Body and Society 28 (1-2):24-59.
    Moulages are contact media – images made by contagion in the most literal sense: their production relies on a process in which the object to be reproduced is touched by the reproducing material. In the case of dermatological moulages, the plaster touches the infected skin of the sick and, once dried, serves as the negative form for the waxen image of a disease. Focussing on the collection of the Hôpital Saint-Louis in Paris, the article situates the production of dermatological (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  13
    Action-space theory of conscious vision.David Ward - 2010 - Dissertation, University of Edinburgh
    I argue that conscious visual experience consists in a direct and noninferential grasp of the way one’s current perceptual contact with the environment poises one to pursue various intentional plans, goals and projects. I show that such a view of visual consciousness is supported by current work in cognitive neuroscience, affords a compelling account of colour perception, and suggests a way to bridge the ‘explanatory gap’ between consciousness and the language of the natural sciences. In chapter 1, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  75
    Principles of object perception.Elizabeth S. Spelke - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):29--56.
    Research on human infants has begun to shed light on early-developing processes for segmenting perceptual arrays into objects. Infants appear to perceive objects by analyzing three-dimensional surface arrangements and motions. Their perception does not accord with a general tendency to maximize figural goodness or to attend to nonaccidental geometric relations in visual arrays. Object perception does accord with principles governing the motions of material bodies: Infants divide perceptual arrays into units that move as connected wholes, that move separately from (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   286 citations  
  50. Marr on computational-level theories.Oron Shagrir - 2010 - Philosophy of Science 77 (4):477-500.
    According to Marr, a computational-level theory consists of two elements, the what and the why . This article highlights the distinct role of the Why element in the computational analysis of vision. Three theses are advanced: ( a ) that the Why element plays an explanatory role in computational-level theories, ( b ) that its goal is to explain why the computed function (specified by the What element) is appropriate for a given visual task, and ( c ) that (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000