Results for 'orienting task'

993 found
Order:
  1.  14
    Orienting task and study time in facial recognition.John H. Mueller, Michael Carlomusto & Alvin G. Goldstein - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11 (5):313-316.
  2.  10
    Strategy Generalization Across Orientation Tasks: Testing a Computational Cognitive Model.Glenn Gunzelmann - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (5):835-861.
    Humans use their spatial information processing abilities flexibly to facilitate problem solving and decision making in a variety of tasks. This article explores the question of whether a general strategy can be adapted for performing two different spatial orientation tasks by testing the predictions of a computational cognitive model. Human performance was measured on an orientation task requiring participants to identify the location of a target either on a map (find‐on‐map) or within an egocentric view of a space (find‐in‐scene). (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  3.  29
    Effects of orienting task, practice, and incentive on simultaneous incidental and intentional learning.Arnold Mechanic - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (4):393.
  4.  24
    Association value and orienting task in incidental and intentional paired-associate learning.Frank W. Wicker & Alan L. Bernstein - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (2):308.
  5. Effects of orienting task and presentation modality on judged frequency.C. G. Penney - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (5):352-352.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  26
    Anxiety and orienting tasks in picture recognition.John H. Mueller, David J. Miller & Jeffrey L. Hutchings - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 13 (3):145-148.
  7.  24
    Effects of orienting tasks and instructions about associative structure on free recall and clustering.Robert E. Till, Carroll D. Johnston & James J. Jenkins - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (4):349-351.
  8.  27
    When do semantic orienting tasks hinder recall?Eugene Winograd & Anderson D. Smith - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11 (3):165-167.
  9.  13
    Effects of instructions, orienting task, and memory tests on memory for words and word frequency.James R. Erickson & Carol Renaud Gaffney - 1985 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23 (4):377-380.
  10.  5
    Enhanced source memory for emotionally valenced sources: does an affective orienting task make the difference?Nikoletta Symeonidou & Beatrice G. Kuhlmann - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Previous research on whether source memory is enhanced for emotionally valenced sources yielded inconclusive results. To identify potential boundary conditions, we tested whether encoding instructions that promote affective versus different types of non-affective item-source-processing foster versus hamper source-valence effects. In both experiments, we used neutral words as items superimposed on emotional (positive & negative) or neutral pictures as sources. Source pictures were selected based on valence and arousal ratings collected in a pre-study such that only valence varied across sources. Source (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  40
    Differential effects of effort and type of orienting task on recall and organization of highly associated words.Thomas S. Hyde - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 97 (1):111.
  12.  23
    Studies in incidental learning: IV. The interaction of orienting tasks and stimulus materials.Leo Postman & Pauline Austin Adams - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (5):329.
  13.  16
    Visual and pronouncing responses, and the relation between orienting task and presentations in incidental learning.Arnold Mechanic & Joanne D'Andrea - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (3):343.
  14. Recognition memory and awareness: A large effect of study-test modalities on "know" responses following a highly perceptual orienting task.V. H. Gregg & John M. Gardiner - 1994 - European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 6:137-47.
  15.  45
    Attentional asymmetries in a visual orienting task are related to temperament.Kelly G. Garner, Paul E. Dux, Joe Wagner, D. R. Tarrant, Christopher D. Chambers & A. Mark - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (8):1508-1515.
    Spatial asymmetries are an intriguing feature of directed attention. Recent observations indicate an influence of temperament upon the direction of these asymmetries. It is unknown whether this influence generalises to visual orienting behaviour. The aim of the current study was therefore to explore the relationship between temperament and measures of spatial orienting as a function of target hemifield. An exogenous cueing task was administered to 92 healthy participants. Temperament was assessed using Carver and White's (1994) Behavioural Inhibition (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  16.  17
    Attentional prioritization reconfigures novel instructions into action-oriented task sets.Carlos González-García, Silvia Formica, Baptist Liefooghe & Marcel Brass - 2020 - Cognition 194 (C):104059.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  17.  30
    Intentional learning and retention of words following various orienting tasks.Peter C. P. Chow, Janice L. Currie & Fergus I. M. Craik - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12 (2):109-112.
  18.  14
    Effects of semantic and perceptual orienting tasks on preschool children’s memory.Marion Perlmutter, Edward J. Schork & Denise Lewis - 1982 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 19 (2):65-68.
  19.  17
    Attentional asymmetries in a visual orienting task are related to temperament.Kelly G. Garner, Paul E. Dux, Joe Wagner, Tarrant D. R. Cummins, Christopher D. Chambers & Mark A. Bellgrove - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (8):1508-1515.
  20.  18
    Orienting in virtual environments: How are surface features and environmental geometry weighted in an orientation task?Debbie M. Kelly & Walter F. Bischof - 2008 - Cognition 109 (1):89-104.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  21.  2
    Speakers’ orientations to directional terms in a map task.Anna Filipi - 2014 - Discourse Studies 16 (3):365-384.
    Drawing on a corpus of eight task-based interactions involving a map task ), this article seeks to focus on how speakers orient to directional terms in an interactional context, and in so doing, to add to features that can be deemed to characterize this speech exchange system. Analysis of the data using conversation analysis showed that features of the talk were confirmation and clarification checks, which provided an opportunity to check understanding and the accuracy of the instruction leading (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  33
    Attentional orienting and awareness: Evidence from a discrimination task.María Fernanda López-Ramón, Ana B. Chica, Paolo Bartolomeo & Juan Lupiáñez - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (3):745-755.
    We used several cue–target SOAs and three different degrees of cue predictability , to investigate the role of awareness of cue–target predictability on cueing effects. A group of participants received instructions about the informative value of the cue, while another group did not receive such instructions. Participants were able to extract the predictive value of a spatially peripheral cue and use it to orient attention, whether or not specific instructions about the predictive value of the cue were given, and no (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  23.  11
    Task Dependent Effects of Head Orientation on Perceived Gaze Direction.Tarryn Balsdon & Colin W. G. Clifford - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  10
    A task-oriented taxonomy of visual completion.Carol Yin - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (6):780-781.
    Differences and similarities between modal and amodal completions can only be understood by considering the goals of visual completion: unity, shape, and perceptual quality. Pessoa et al. cannot reject representational accounts of vision because of flaws with isomorphic representations of perceptual quality: representations and processes for perceptual quality (modal completion) and most likely dissociable from those for unity and shape (nonmodal completions).
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  3
    Diagnosis of Oriental Ethics Education in the Middle School and Its Tasks. 이상호 - 2013 - Journal of Ethics: The Korean Association of Ethics 1 (92):1-18.
  26.  40
    A spatially oriented decision does not induce consciousness in a motor task.Bruce Bridgeman & Valerie Huemer - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):454-464.
    Visual information follows at least two branches in the human nervous system, following a common input stage: a cognitive ''what'' branch governs perception and experience, while a sensorimotor ''how'' branch handles visually guided behavior though its outputs are unconscious. The sensorimotor system is probed with an isomorphic task, requiring a 1:1 relationship between target position and motor response. The cognitive system, in contrast, is probed with a forced qualitative decision, expressed verbally, about the location of a target. Normally, the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  27.  25
    Making a task difficult: Evidence that device-oriented steps are effortful and error-prone.Maartje Ga Ament, Anna L. Cox, Ann Blandford & Duncan P. Brumby - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 19 (3):195.
  28.  21
    A Study of the Mechanism of the Congruence of Leader–Follower Power Distance Orientation on Employees’ Task Performance.Yan Bao, Shudi Liao, Jianqiao Liao, Yucheng Zhang, Chuanjun Deng & Zhiwen Guo - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:441710.
    Based on implicit leadership theory, we examine the congruence effect of leader-follower power distance orientation on follower trust in a supervisor and work engagement, which in turn influences employees’ task performance. Results of Polynomial regressions on 526 dyads supported the congruence effect hypothesis. The results show that (1) the congruence of leader-follower power distance orientation leads to better performance; (2) under the condition of congruence, Subordinate task performance is higher when leader-follower power distance orientation matching in low-low ratings (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  19
    Implicit attentional orienting in a target detection task with central cues.Scott A. Peterson & Tanja N. Gibson - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1532-1547.
    Studies using Posner’s spatial cueing paradigm have demonstrated that participants can allocate their attention to specific target locations based on the predictiveness of preceding cues. Four experiments were conducted to investigate attentional orienting processes operating in a high probability condition as compared to a low probability condition using various types of centrally-presented cues. Spatially-informative cues resulted in cueing effects for both probability conditions, with significantly larger CEs in the high probability conditions than the low probability conditions. Participants in the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  30.  31
    Beyond intersubjectivity: Task orientation and first language use in foreign language discussions.Eric Hauser - 2013 - Pragmatics and Society 4 (3):285-316.
    One type of task interaction that students in a foreign language class may do is using the language they are studying for discussion. This paper analyzes interaction among Japanese university students participating in such discussions in English. The participants are interactionally competent; one source of resources they draw on to construct this competence is their first language, Japanese. Participants occasionally use Japanese to refer to Japanese things. They also use Japanese in the pursuit of intersubjectivity, such as using Japanese (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31. Moving from task‐oriented to learning‐oriented strategies on school excursions to museums.Janette Griffin & David Symington - 1997 - Science Education 81 (6):763-779.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  32.  7
    The Timing of Utterance Planning in Task-Oriented Dialogue: Evidence from a Novel List-Completion Paradigm.Barthel Mathias, Sauppe Sebastian, C. Levinson Stephen & S. Meyer Antje - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  33.  12
    Variations in lecturer task orientation and student perceptions of course effectiveness.Allen J. Schuh - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11 (3):193-194.
  34. Reflexive and voluntary orienting in detection and discrimination tasks.R. Egly, R. D. Rafal & A. Henik - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (6):469-469.
  35.  14
    Control of spatial orienting: Context-specific proportion cued effects in an exogenous spatial cueing task.Alex Gough, Jesse Garcia, Maryem Torres-Quesada & Bruce Milliken - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 30:220-233.
  36.  38
    Visibility Is Not Equivalent to Confidence in a Low Contrast Orientation Discrimination Task.Manuel Rausch & Michael Zehetleitner - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
  37.  24
    An experimental study of the role of the ego in work. II. The significance of task-orientation in work.H. B. Lewis & M. Franklin - 1944 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 34 (3):195.
  38.  8
    The influence of visual and vestibular orientation cues in a clock reading task.Nicolas Davidenko, Yeram Cheong, Amanda Waterman, Jacob Smith, Barrett Anderson & Sarah Harmon - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 64:196-206.
  39.  10
    An empirical assessment of a modified artificially intelligent device use acceptance model—From the task-oriented perspective.Yutao Yang, Jia Luo & Tian Lan - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Artificial intelligence is a cutting-edge technology that has been widely applied in tourism operations. To enhance tourists' experience, many tourism suppliers introduced AI devices to interact with tourists. Previous studies classified AI devices as task- and social- oriented based on their functions; however, current models that explain customers' intention to use AI devices did not reflect the discrepancy between the two different types. Therefore, this paper attempts to fill this gap by proposing a theoretical model for the use of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  10
    Autonomous vs. heteronomous mode of action control and task performance: The role of the situational context and action vs. state orientation.Romana Kadzikowska-Wrzosek - 2015 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 46 (3):433-446.
    The article presents the results of two experimental studies in which I investigated the effect of the situational context and action vs. state orientation on perseverance and efficacy in task performance. The results of Study 1 confirmed that in a context which supports autonomy - as opposed to one which induces external control - people are much more likely to be not only more persistent and effective in their actions but also much more interested in the performed task. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  13
    The Place of the near Orient in the Career of Man and the Task of the American Orientalist.James Henry Breasted - 1919 - Journal of the American Oriental Society 39:159-184.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  25
    Hand and Grasp Selection in a Preferential Reaching Task: The Effects of Object Location, Orientation, and Task Intention.Sara M. Scharoun, Kelly A. Scanlan & Pamela J. Bryden - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  43.  4
    Mechanism design for automated negotiation, and its application to task oriented domains.Gilad Zlotkin & Jeffrey S. Rosenschein - 1996 - Artificial Intelligence 86 (2):195-244.
  44.  50
    Object Orientation Affects Spatial Language Comprehension.Michele Burigo & Simona Sacchi - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (8):1471-1492.
    Typical spatial descriptions, such as “The car is in front of the house,” describe the position of a located object (LO; e.g., the car) in space relative to a reference object (RO) whose location is known (e.g., the house). The orientation of the RO affects spatial language comprehension via the reference frame selection process. However, the effects of the LO's orientation on spatial language have not received great attention. This study explores whether the pure geometric information of the LO (e.g., (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  45.  10
    Response to Commentary: Hand and Grasp Selection in a Preferential Reaching Task: The Effects of Object Location, Orientation, and Task Intention.Sara M. Scharoun Benson & Pamela J. Bryden - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  33
    Corrigendum: The Binocular Balance at High Spatial Frequencies as Revealed by the Binocular Orientation Combination Task.Yonghua Wang, Zhifen He, Yunjie Liang, Yiya Chen, Ling Gong, Yu Mao, Xiaoxin Chen, Zhimo Yao, Daniel P. Spiegel, Jia Qu, Fan Lu, Jiawei Zhou & Robert F. Hess - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  47.  37
    The Binocular Balance at High Spatial Frequencies as Revealed by the Binocular Orientation Combination Task.Yonghua Wang, Zhifen He, Yunjie Liang, Yiya Chen, Ling Gong, Yu Mao, Xiaoxin Chen, Zhimo Yao, Daniel P. Spiegel, Jia Qu, Fan Lu, Jiawei Zhou & Robert F. Hess - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  48. Displaying Recipiency: Reactive Tokens in Mandarin Task-Oriented Interaction.[author unknown] - 2016
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49. Tasks of Philosophy in the Present Age RIAS-Lecture, June 9, 1952.Cynthia R. Nielsen & Ian Alexander Moore - 2020 - Philosophy Today 64 (2):1-8.
    Translators’ Abstract: This is a translation of Hans-Georg Gadamer’s recently discovered 1952 Berlin speech. The speech includes several themes that reappear in Truth and Method, as well as in Gadamer’s later writings such as Reason in the Age of Science. For example, Gadamer criticizes positivism, modern philosophy’s orientation toward positivism, and Enlightenment narratives of progress, while presenting his view of philosophy’s tasks in an age of crisis. In addition, he discusses structural power, instrumental reason, the objectification of nature and human (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  59
    Orienting of attention via observed eye gaze is head-centred.Andrew P. Bayliss, Giuseppe di Pellegrino & Steven P. Tipper - 2004 - Cognition 94 (1):1-10.
    Observing averted eye gaze results in the automatic allocation of attention to the gazed-at location. The role of the orientation of the face that produces the gaze cue was investigated. The eyes in the face could look left or right in a head-centred frame, but the face itself could be oriented 90 degrees clockwise or anticlockwise such that the eyes were gazing up or down. Significant cueing effects to targets presented to the left or right of the screen were found (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
1 — 50 / 993