Results for 'Spatial systems'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Bodily Systems and the Spatial-Functional Structure of the Human Body.Barry Smith - 2004 - Studies in Health and Technology Informatics 102:39–63.
    The human body is a system made of systems. The body is divided into bodily systems proper, such as the endocrine and circulatory systems, which are subdivided into many sub-systems at a variety of levels, whereby all systems and subsystems engage in massive causal interaction with each other and with their surrounding environments. Here we offer an explicit definition of bodily system and provide a framework for understanding their causal interactions. Medical sciences provide at best (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  2.  5
    Spatial temporal information systems: an ontological approach using STK.Linda M. McNeil - 2013 - Boca Raton: CRC Press. Edited by T. S. Kelso.
    Designed to be a high-level, approachable resource for engineers who need further insight into spatial temporal information systems from an ontological perspective, Spatial Temporal Information Systems: An Ontological Approach using STK® explains the dynamics of objects interaction from signal analysis to trajectory design, spatial modeling, and other spatial analytics by using STK®, which is a general-purpose modeling and analysis application for any type of space, defense, or intelligence system. Building a foundation to begin the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  21
    Spatializing Emotion: No Evidence for a Domain‐General Magnitude System.Benjamin Pitt & Daniel Casasanto - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (7):2150-2180.
    People implicitly associate different emotions with different locations in left-right space. Which aspects of emotion do they spatialize, and why? Across many studies people spatialize emotional valence, mapping positive emotions onto their dominant side of space and negative emotions onto their non-dominant side, consistent with theories of metaphorical mental representation. Yet other results suggest a conflicting mapping of emotional intensity (a.k.a., emotional magnitude), according to which people associate more intense emotions with the right and less intense emotions with the left (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  4.  48
    Relational proof systems for spatial reasoning.Joanna Golińska-Pilarek & Ewa Orlowska - 2006 - Journal of Applied Non-Classical Logics 16 (3-4):409-431.
    We present relational proof systems for the four groups of theories of spatial reasoning: contact relation algebras, Boolean algebras with a contact relation, lattice-based spatial theories, spatial theories based on a proximity relation.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  48
    Spatial coordinates and phenomenology in the two-visual systems model.Pierre Jacob & Frédérique de Vignemont - 2010 - In Nivedita Gangopadhyay, Michael Madary & Finn Spicer (eds.), Perception, Action, and Consciousness: Sensorimotor Dynamics and Two Visual Systems. New York: Oxford University Press.
  6. On the Spatial Foundations of the Conceptual System and Its Enrichment.Jean M. Mandler - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (3):421-451.
    A theory of how concept formation begins is presented that accounts for conceptual activity in the first year of life, shows how increasing conceptual complexity comes about, and predicts the order in which new types of information accrue to the conceptual system. In a compromise between nativist and empiricist views, it offers a single domain-general mechanism that redescribes attended spatiotemporal information into an iconic form. The outputs of this mechanism consist of types of spatial information that we know infants (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  7.  38
    Spatial symbol systems and spatial cognition: A computer science perspective on perception-based symbol processing.Christian Freksa, Thomas Barkowsky & Alexander Klippel - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):616-617.
    People often solve spatially presented cognitive problems more easily than their nonspatial counterparts. We explain this phenomenon by characterizing space as an inter-modality that provides common structure to different specific perceptual modalities. The usefulness of spatial structure for knowledge processing on different levels of granularity and for interaction between internal and external processes is described. Map representations are discussed as examples in which the usefulness of spatially organized symbols is particularly evident. External representations and processes can enhance internal representations (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  23
    Spatial complexity of character-based writing systems and arithmetic in primary school: a longitudinal study.Maja Rodic, Tatiana Tikhomirova, Tatiana Kolienko, Sergey Malykh, Olga Bogdanova, Dina Y. Zueva, Elena I. Gynku, Sirui Wan, Xinlin Zhou & Yulia Kovas - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  9. Spatial and nonspatial avenues to object recognition by the human haptic system.Roberta L. Klatzky & Susan J. Lederman - 1993 - In Naomi Eilan, Rosaleen A. McCarthy & Bill Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation: problems in philosophy and psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell. pp. 191--205.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10.  43
    Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability.Marcel A. Just & Patricia A. Carpenter - 1985 - Psychological Review 92 (2):137-172.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  11.  8
    Dual Systems for Spatial Updating in Immediate and Retrieved Environments: Evidence from Bias Analysis.Chuanjun Liu & Chengli Xiao - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  12.  47
    Embodied Spatial Cognition.J. Gregory Trafton & Anthony M. Harrison - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (4):686-706.
    We present a spatial system called Specialized Egocentrically Coordinated Spaces embedded in an embodied cognitive architecture (ACT-R Embodied). We show how the spatial system works by modeling two different developmental findings: gaze-following and Level 1 perspective taking. The gaze-following model is based on an experiment by Corkum and Moore (1998), whereas the Level 1 visual perspective-taking model is based on an experiment by Moll and Tomasello (2006). The models run on an embodied robotic system.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  13.  28
    Embodiment, spatial categorisation and action.Yann Coello & Yvonne Delevoye-Turrell - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (3):667-683.
    Despite the subjective experience of a continuous and coherent external world, we will argue that the perception and categorisation of visual space is constrained by the spatial resolution of the sensory systems but also and above all, by the pre-reflective representations of the body in action. Recent empirical data in cognitive neurosciences will be presented that suggest that multidimensional categorisation of perceptual space depends on body representations at both an experiential and a functional level. Results will also be (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  14. Egocentric Spatial Representation in Action and Perception.Robert Briscoe - 2009 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 79 (2):423-460.
    Neuropsychological findings used to motivate the "two visual systems" hypothesis have been taken to endanger a pair of widely accepted claims about spatial representation in conscious visual experience. The first is the claim that visual experience represents 3-D space around the perceiver using an egocentric frame of reference. The second is the claim that there is a constitutive link between the spatial contents of visual experience and the perceiver's bodily actions. In this paper, I review and assess (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  15.  10
    A coding system for spatial relational reference.Chris Sinha & Lis A. Thorseng - 1995 - Cognitive Linguistics 6 (2-3):261-309.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  16.  10
    Modified Navigation Instructions for Spatial Navigation Assistance Systems Lead to Incidental Spatial Learning.Klaus Gramann, Paul Hoepner & Katja Karrer-Gauss - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
  17.  7
    Piaget's system of spatial logic: The semiosis of index.Donna E. West - 2014 - Semiotica 2014 (202).
    Name der Zeitschrift: Semiotica Jahrgang: 2014 Heft: 202 Seiten: 459-480.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  12
    Repositioning Universities in Multi‐spatial Innovation Systems: The Japanese Case.Fumi Kitagawa - 2006 - Social Epistemology 20 (3 & 4):299 – 314.
    Universities are increasingly part of wider geographical processes including international, national and sub-national actors. Universities now find themselves having to pay attention to many more political centres than before, as seen, for example, with research grants, assessments and teaching accreditation from transnational bodies, individual states and regional authorities. As an institution, the university constitutes a place which needs to be situated within a wider space and the geography of power-relations. This article traces these spatial developments in relation to recent (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  14
    Chaotic dynamics in a spatially extended magnetic system: A Bloch wall between two domains.A. Sukiennicki & J. J. Zebrowski - 1995 - In R. J. Russell, N. Murphy & A. R. Peacocke (eds.), Chaos and Complexity. Vatican Observatory Publications. pp. 261.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  48
    Spatial Representations Elicit Dual‐Coding Effects in Mental Imagery.Michelle Verges & Sean Duffy - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (6):1157-1172.
    Spatial aspects of words are associated with their canonical locations in the real world. Yet little research has tested whether spatial associations denoted in language comprehension generalize to their corresponding images. We directly tested the spatial aspects of mental imagery in picture and word processing (Experiment 1). We also tested whether spatial representations of motion words produce similar perceptual-interference effects as demonstrated by object words (Experiment 2). Findings revealed that words denoting an upward spatial location (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  21.  16
    Spatially Resolved Transcriptomes—Next Generation Tools for Tissue Exploration.Michaela Asp, Joseph Bergenstråhle & Joakim Lundeberg - 2020 - Bioessays 42 (10):1900221.
    Recent advances in spatially resolved transcriptomics have greatly expanded the knowledge of complex multicellular biological systems. The field has quickly expanded in recent years, and several new technologies have been developed that all aim to combine gene expression data with spatial information. The vast array of methodologies displays fundamental differences in their approach to obtain this information, and thus, demonstrate method‐specific advantages and shortcomings. While the field is moving forward at a rapid pace, there are still multiple challenges (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  8
    Temporally and spatially flexible plan execution for dynamic hybrid systems.Andreas G. Hofmann & Brian C. Williams - 2017 - Artificial Intelligence 247 (C):266-294.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  23.  7
    Stationary and time-dependent spatial patterns in a chemical system.P. de Kepper, B. Rudovics, Jj Perraud & E. Dulos - 1995 - In R. J. Russell, N. Murphy & A. R. Peacocke (eds.), Chaos and Complexity. Vatican Observatory Publications.
  24.  9
    Spatial and Temporal Reasoning.Oliviero Stock (ed.) - 1997 - Kluwer Academic Publishers.
    Qualitative reasoning about space and time - a reasoning at the human level - promises to become a fundamental aspect of future systems that will accompany us in daily activity. The aim of Spatial and Temporal Reasoning is to give a picture of current research in this area focusing on both representational and computational issues. The picture emphasizes some major lines of development in this multifaceted, constantly growing area. The material in the book also shows some common ground (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  25. Two neural systems for visual orienting and the pathophysiology of unilateral spatial neglect.M. Corbetta, M. J. Kincade & G. L. Shulman - 2002 - In Hans-Otto Karnath, David Milner & Giuseppe Vallar (eds.), The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect. Oxford University Press. pp. 259--273.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  9
    The dual systems in temporal cognition: A spatial analogy.Andrea Roselli - 2019 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42.
    The model presented by the authors can explain an inherent contradiction in people's naïve theory of time. In this commentary I suggest a way in which another paradox of our phenomenal temporality may be addressed along these lines. In the final section, I also discuss some concerns that may arise about the clear-cut distinction between humans and non-human animals.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  14
    The Spatial Learning Task of Lhermitte and Signoret (1972): Normative Data in Adults Aged 18–45.Alana Collins, Michael M. Saling, Sarah J. Wilson, Graeme D. Jackson & Chris Tailby - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13:860982.
    ObjectiveThe Spatial Learning Task of Lhermitte and Signoret is an object-location arbitrary associative learning task. The task was originally developed to evaluate adults with severe amnesia. It is currently used in populations where the memory system either is not yet fully developed or where it has been compromised (e.g. epilepsy, traumatic brain injury, electroconvulsive therapy, cerebrovascular disease and dementia). Normative data have been published for paediatric cohorts and for older adults, however no data exist for the intervening adult years.MethodHere, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  16
    The Seeds of Spatial Grammar in the Manual Modality.Wing Chee So, Marie Coppola, Vincent Licciardello & Susan Goldin-Meadow - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (6):1029-1043.
    Sign languages modulate the production of signs in space and use this spatial modulation to refer back to entities—to maintain coreference. We ask here whether spatial modulation is so fundamental to language in the manual modality that it will be invented by individuals asked to create gestures on the spot. English speakers were asked to describe vignettes under 2 conditions: using gesture without speech, and using speech with spontaneous gestures. When using gesture alone, adults placed gestures for particular (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  29.  28
    Scattering from Spatially Localized Chaotic and Disordered Systems.L. E. Reichl & G. Akguc - 2001 - Foundations of Physics 31 (2):243-267.
    A version of scattering theory that was developed many years ago to treat nuclear scattering processes, has provided a powerful tool to study universality in scattering processes involving open quantum systems with underlying classically chaotic dynamics. Recently, it has been used to make random matrix theory predictions concerning the statistical properties of scattering resonances in mesoscopic electron waveguides and electromagnetic waveguides. We provide a simple derivation of this scattering theory and we compare its predictions to those obtained from an (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  13
    The effects of spatial stability and cue type on spatial learning: Implications for theories of parallel memory systems.Matthew G. Buckley, Joe M. Austen, Liam A. M. Myles, Shamus Smith, Niklas Ihssen, Adina R. Lew & Anthony McGregor - 2021 - Cognition 214 (C):104802.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  11
    Material Spatialities of Cities and States.Peter J. Taylor - 2004 - ProtoSociology 20:30-45.
    The concept of spatiality is introduced as an analytical tool for studying the modern world-system. The spatialities of cities and states are contrasted as spaces of flows and spaces of places respectively. It is argued that the latter is embedded in the social sciences as an unexamined spatiality. Dis-embedding is achieved through constructing a revisionist world-systems analysis that focuses on cities. This world-systems analysis is then used to describe two world-spatialities, for the current situation and for a generation (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  9
    Ecological Location of a Water Source and Spatial Dynamics of Behavior Under Temporally Scheduled Water Deliveries in a Modified Open-Field System: An Integrative Approach.Alejandro León, Varsovia Hernández, Ursula Huerta, Carlos Alberto Hernández-Linares, Porfirio Toledo, Martha Lorena Avendaño Garrido, Esteban Escamilla Navarro & Isiris Guzmán - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    It has been reported in non-contingent schedules that the variety of patterns of behavior is affected by the temporal variation of water deliveries. While temporal variation is accomplished by delivering water at fixed or variable times, spatial variation is usually accomplished by varying the number of dispensers and distance among them. Such criteria do not consider the possible ecological relevance of the location of water dispensers. Nevertheless, it is plausible to suppose that the intersection of the programed contingencies, the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  14
    Research on the Impact of Chinese Total Factor Productivity on the Modern Economic System Based on the Spatial Dubin Model.Qin Tang, Zhi-An Ren, Kang-Feng Zhu & Nai-Ru Xu - 2021 - Complexity 2021:1-14.
    Total factor productivity is not only the core of high-quality economic development but also a core indicator for measuring the quality of economic development. Improving total factor productivity is one of the most critical points in building a modern economic system. Firstly, this paper uses the DEA-Malmquist index method to measure and decompose the total factor productivity of China’s 30 provinces, municipalities, and autonomous regions from 2001 to 2017 and analyzes the characteristics of its temporal and spatial changes. From (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  37
    Spatial Semantics, Cognition, and Their Interaction: A Comparative Study of Spatial Categorization in English and Korean.Hongoak Yun & Soonja Choi - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (6):1736-1776.
    This study has two goals. First, we present much‐needed empirical linguistic data and systematic analyses on the spatial semantic systems in English and Korean, two languages that have been extensively compared to date in the debate on spatial language and spatial cognition. We conduct our linguistic investigation comprehensively, encompassing the domains of tight‐ and loose‐fit as well as containment and support relations. The current analysis reveals both cross‐linguistic commonalities and differences: From a common set of (...) features, each language highlights a subset of those features for its principal categorization, and those primary features are importantly different between English and Korean: English speakers categorize events predominantly by containment and support relations (and do so with prepositions), whereas Korean speakers categorize them by tight‐fit and loose‐fit relations (and do so with verbs), with a further distinction of containment and support within the loose‐fit relation. The analysis also shows that the tight‐fit domain is more cross‐linguistically diverse in categorization than is the loose‐fit domain. Second, we test the language data against the nonlinguistic categorization results reported in Choi and Hattrup (2012). The results show a remarkable degree of convergence between the patterns predicted from the current linguistic analysis and those found in C&H's nonlinguistic study and thus provide empirical and strong evidence for an influence of language on nonlinguistic spatial cognition. At the same time, the study reveals areas where the two systems closely interact with each other as well as those where one is independent from the other. Taking both parts of the study together, we identify the specific roles that language and spatial perception/cognition play in spatial categorization. (shrink)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  35.  60
    Spatial deictic tense and evidentials in Korean.Kyung-Sook Chung - 2007 - Natural Language Semantics 15 (3):187-219.
    This paper focuses on the Korean suffix -te, which has been variously analyzed as a marker of tense, aspect, tense–aspect, mood, mood–tense, or evidentiality. I argue against all of these approaches and propose instead that -te is a spatial deictic past tense, which triggers an evidential environment. It refers to a certain past time when the speaker either observed an event or some evidence of the event within his (her) perceptual field. Thus, the denotation of -te is ‘overlap’, not (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  36.  14
    Innate and Cultural Spatial Time: A Developmental Perspective.Barbara Magnani & Alessandro Musetti - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
    We reviewed literature to understand when a spatial map for time is available in the brain. We carefully defined the concepts of metrical map of time and of conceptual representation of time as the mental time line (MTL) in order to formulate our position. It is that both metrical map and conceptual representation of time are spatial in nature. The former should be innate, related to motor/implicit timing, it should represent all magnitudes with an analogic and bi-dimensional structure. (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  37.  36
    A spatial coding analysis of the a-not-b error: What IS “location at a”?Nora S. Newcombe - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1):57-58.
    Thelen et al. criticize “spatial coding” approaches to the A-not-B error. However, newer thinking about spatial coding provides more precise analytic categories and recognizes that different spatial coding systems normally coexist. Theorizing about spatial coding is largely compatible with dynamic-systems theory, augmenting it with an analysis of what one means when discussing “location at A” (or B).
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  65
    Attention to Endpoints: A Cross‐Linguistic Constraint on Spatial Meaning.Terry Regier & Mingyu Zheng - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (4):705-719.
    We investigate a possible universal constraint on spatial meaning. It has been proposed that people attend preferentially to the endpoints of spatial motion events, and that languages may therefore make finer semantic distinctions at event endpoints than at event beginnings. We test this proposal. In Experiment 1, we show that people discriminate the endpoints of spatial motion events more readily than they do event beginnings—suggesting a non-linguistic attentional bias toward endpoints. In Experiment 2, speakers of Arabic, Chinese, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  39.  11
    Support for a memory – not spatial – deficit after hippocampal system damage.John A. Walker - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):348-349.
  40.  77
    “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition.Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):217-238.
    Fundamental to spatial knowledge in all species are the representations underlying object recognition, object search, and navigation through space. But what sets humans apart from other species is our ability to express spatial experience through language. This target article explores the language ofobjectsandplaces, asking what geometric properties are preserved in the representations underlying object nouns and spatial prepositions in English. Evidence from these two aspects of language suggests there are significant differences in the geometric richness with which (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   101 citations  
  41.  13
    Complementary frequency selective surface pair-based intelligent spatial filters for 5G wireless systems.Pradeep Kumar, Ranjan Mishra & Ankush Kapoor - 2021 - Journal of Intelligent Systems 30 (1):1054-1069.
    Frequency selective surface -based intelligent spatial filters are capturing the eyes of the researchers by offering a dynamic behavior when exposed to the electromagnetic radiations. In this manuscript, a concept of creating complementary structures which stems from Babinet’s principle is illustrated. A hybrid complementary pair of FSS comprising double square loop FSS and double square slot FSS on either side of the dielectric substrate is proposed. DSLFSS offers band-pass behavior and can be placed as a superstrate, whereas DSSFSS behaves (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42. The spatial presence of spirits among the cartesians.Jasper William Reid - 2008 - Journal of the History of Philosophy 46 (1):91-117.
    : The Cartesians have often been read as if they denied spatial presence to incorporeal substances, reserving it for extended things alone. This article explores whether this common interpretation is accurate, examining the cases of both created minds and the divine substance of God Himself. Through scrutiny of the relevant texts of both Descartes himself and his followers, it demonstrates that, in the divine case, this common interpretation is incorrect, and that the Cartesians did believe that God’s own substance (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  43.  36
    Spatial Subsystem of Moral Metaphors: A Cognitive Semantic Study.Ning Yu, Tianfang Wang & Yingliang He - 2016 - Metaphor and Symbol 31 (4):195-211.
    Cognitive semantic studies have shown that our conceptualization of morality is at least partially metaphorical and that our moral cognition is grounded in some fundamental contrastive categories of our embodied experience in the physical environment. It is argued that our moral cognition is built on a moral metaphor system. Within the framework of conceptual metaphor theory, this study aims to examine the spatial subsystem of moral metaphors in English. We set out with five pairs of moral metaphors that involve (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  44.  19
    Spatial semiosis and time.Leonid Tchertov - 2005 - Sign Systems Studies 33 (2):297-314.
    Spatial semiosis differs from temporal one by its structural and functional peculiarities. Meaningful relations between units of spatial texts are not ordered along of temporal axe and do not need time in their form of expression. However time remains an important factor for both: being of the spatial semiosis in the external time and being of time in the spatial texts as object of representation. In the contrast to temporal communication, where acts receiving of texts must (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  43
    Spatial semiosis and time.Leonid Tchertov - 2005 - Sign Systems Studies 33 (2):297-314.
    Spatial semiosis differs from temporal one by its structural and functional peculiarities. Meaningful relations between units of spatial texts are not ordered along of temporal axe and do not need time in their form of expression. However time remains an important factor for both: being of the spatial semiosis in the external time and being of time in the spatial texts as object of representation. In the contrast to temporal communication, where acts receiving of texts must (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  11
    Complexity Heliophysics: A [new] system science that transcends the previous boundaries of our field.Ryan McGranaghan, Seebany Datta-Barua, Jeffrey Thayer, Joseph Borovsky, Jay Johnson, Simon Wing, Dan Baker & Massimo Materassi - unknown
    The 21st century is the time of complexity. We delineate it and its importance as necessary to solve ‘wicked problems.’ Inherently transdisciplinary, trans-scale, and interconnected to living systems, the solution to Heliophysics’ identity crisis and to unlock the next generation of scientific discovery may be to embrace complexity. With the right foresight, direction, and incentive over the next ten years, Heliophysics can become a beacon for how all of society thinks about and does complexity science.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  16
    Spatial Categorization and its Relationship with Cognitive Development: A Pilot Study in Chilean Spanish.Carla Rimassa & Sabela Fernández-Silva - 2014 - Alpha (Osorno) 38:137-154.
    En este artículo presentamos un estudio piloto en que se explora la relación entre la conceptualización del espacio y el desarrollo cognitivo. Para ello se pidió a ocho sujetos de cuatro rangos etarios describir la ubicación espacial de los elementos de una fotografía, a fin de identificar los aspectos que diferencian las expresiones referentes a la ubicación espacial entre rangos etarios. Se observa que, en nuestra muestra, la conceptualización del espacio se complejiza conforme avanza la edad, con diferencias en cinco (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  40
    A spatially oriented decision does not induce consciousness in a motor task.Bruce Bridgeman & Valerie Huemer - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):454-464.
    Visual information follows at least two branches in the human nervous system, following a common input stage: a cognitive ''what'' branch governs perception and experience, while a sensorimotor ''how'' branch handles visually guided behavior though its outputs are unconscious. The sensorimotor system is probed with an isomorphic task, requiring a 1:1 relationship between target position and motor response. The cognitive system, in contrast, is probed with a forced qualitative decision, expressed verbally, about the location of a target. Normally, the cognitive (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  49.  23
    Is Emotional Magnitude Spatialized? A Further Investigation.Kevin J. Holmes, Candelaria Alcat & Stella F. Lourenco - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (4):e12727.
    Accumulating evidence suggests that different magnitudes (e.g., number, size, and duration) are spatialized in the mind according to a common left–right metric, consistent with a generalized system for representing magnitude. A previous study conducted by two of us (Holmes & Lourenco, ) provided evidence that this metric extends to the processing of emotional magnitude, or the intensity of emotion expressed in faces. Recently, however, Pitt and Casasanto () showed that the earlier effects may have been driven by a left–right mapping (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  50.  63
    Spatial Degrees of Freedom in Everett Quantum Mechanics.Mark A. Rubin - 2006 - Foundations of Physics 36 (8):1115-1159.
    Stapp claims that, when spatial degrees of freedom are taken into account, Everett quantum mechanics is ambiguous due to a “core basis problem.” To examine an aspect of this claim I generalize the ideal measurement model to include translational degrees of freedom for both the measured system and the measuring apparatus. Analysis of this generalized model using the Everett interpretation in the Heisenberg picture shows that it makes unambiguous predictions for the possible results of measurements and their respective probabilities. (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000