Results for ' reinforced stimuli'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  15
    Effectiveness of secondary reinforcing stimuli as a function of the quantity and quality of food reinforcement.Charles Owen Hopkins - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 50 (5):339.
  2.  19
    Affective visual stimuli as operant reinforcers of the GSR.Gary E. Schwartz & Harold J. Johnson - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (1):28.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  18
    Generalization gradients around stimuli associated with different reinforcement schedules.Norman Guttman - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 58 (5):335.
  4.  13
    Associative Learning of Stimuli Paired and Unpaired With Reinforcement: Evaluating Evidence From Maggots, Flies, Bees, and Rats.Michael Schleyer, Markus Fendt, Sarah Schuller & Bertram Gerber - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  8
    Comparison of the reinforcing properties of conditioned and discriminative stimuli in new and previously experienced environments.J. Dutch - 1974 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (2):85-86.
  6.  23
    Factors affecting the conditioned reinforcing strength of stimuli in differential reinforcement of other behavior and fixed-time schedules.Alexander M. Myers & Edward K. Grossman - 1980 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 16 (1):27-30.
  7.  29
    A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement.N. J. Mackintosh - 1975 - Psychological Review 82 (4):276-298.
  8.  10
    Resistance to extinction as a function of partial reinforcement and external stimuli: A within- S design.A. Grant Young & C. A. Costelloe - 1974 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 3 (3):191-192.
  9.  9
    Early vibratory experience and the question of innate reinforcement value of vibration and other stimuli: A limitation on the discrepancy (burnt soup) principle in motivation.J. Mcv Hunt & Herbert C. Quay - 1961 - Psychological Review 68 (3):149-156.
  10.  17
    Goal events as discriminative stimuli over extended intertrial intervals.Martin Pschirrer - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 96 (2):425.
  11.  25
    Predictive Movements and Human Reinforcement Learning of Sequential Action.Roy de Kleijn, George Kachergis & Bernhard Hommel - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S3):783-808.
    Sequential action makes up the bulk of human daily activity, and yet much remains unknown about how people learn such actions. In one motor learning paradigm, the serial reaction time (SRT) task, people are taught a consistent sequence of button presses by cueing them with the next target response. However, the SRT task only records keypress response times to a cued target, and thus it cannot reveal the full time‐course of motion, including predictive movements. This paper describes a mouse movement (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  12.  22
    Effects of intermittent reinforcement of an irrelevant dimension and task complexity upon concept identification.Lyle E. Bourne & Robert C. Haygood - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (6):371.
  13.  15
    Predictive Movements and Human Reinforcement Learning of Sequential Action.Roy Kleijn, George Kachergis & Bernhard Hommel - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S3):783-808.
    Sequential action makes up the bulk of human daily activity, and yet much remains unknown about how people learn such actions. In one motor learning paradigm, the serial reaction time (SRT) task, people are taught a consistent sequence of button presses by cueing them with the next target response. However, the SRT task only records keypress response times to a cued target, and thus it cannot reveal the full time‐course of motion, including predictive movements. This paper describes a mouse movement (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14.  20
    Information value and stimulus configuring as factors in conditioned reinforcement.David R. Thomas, David L. Berman & George E. Serednesky - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (2p1):181.
  15.  13
    The properties of stimuli associated with shock reduction.M. D. Nefzger - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (3):184.
  16.  13
    Discrimination of rewards as a function of contrast in reward stimuli.Mark A. Berkley - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (4):371.
  17.  12
    Magnitude of response to compounds of discriminated stimuli.William W. Grings & Dale E. O'Donnell - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 52 (6):354.
  18.  33
    Hedonic value of intentional action provides reinforcement for voluntary generation but not voluntary inhibition of action.Jim Parkinson & Patrick Haggard - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (4):1253-1261.
    Intentional inhibition refers to stopping oneself from performing an action at the last moment, a vital component of self-control. It has been suggested that intentional inhibition is associated with negative hedonic value, perhaps due to the frustration of cancelling an intended action. Here we investigate hedonic implications of the free choice to act or inhibit. Participants gave aesthetic ratings of arbitrary visual stimuli that immediately followed voluntary decisions to act or to inhibit action. We found that participants for whom (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  80
    Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Delay-of-reinforcement gradients and other behavioral mechanisms.A. Charles Catania - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (3):419-424.
    Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase, and Russell (Sagvolden et al.) examine attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) at levels of analysis ranging from neurotransmitters to behavior. At the behavioral level they attribute aspects of ADHD to anomalies of delay-of-reinforcement gradients. With a normal gradient, responses followed after a long delay by a reinforcer may share in the effects of that reinforcer; with a diminished or steepened gradient they may fail to do so. Steepened gradients differentially select rapidly emitted responses (hyperactivity), and they limit the effectiveness (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  8
    Discrimination learning as a function of pretraining reinforcement schedules.Harold W. Stevenson & Leo A. Pirojnikoff - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 56 (1):41.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  7
    The effect on extinction responding of stimuli continuously present during conditioning.Charles B. Ferster - 1951 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 42 (6):443.
  22.  19
    Stimulus generalization after equal training on two stimuli.Harry I. Kalish & Norman Guttman - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (2):139.
  23.  11
    Preconditioning the effects of shock-correlated reinforcement.John G. Carlson - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (3):409.
  24.  24
    A reversed pre.William B. Pavlik, Peter L. Carlton & Robert Lehr - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (2):274.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  25.  24
    Awareness is awareness is awareness? Decomposing different aspects of awareness and their role in operant learning of pain sensitivity.Susanne Becker, Dieter Kleinböhl & Rupert Hölzl - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (3):1073-1084.
    Regarding awareness as a consistent concept has contributed to the controversy about implicit learning. The present study emphasized the importance of distinguishing aspects of awareness in order to determine whether learning is implicit. By decomposing awareness into awareness of contingencies, of the procedure being a learning task, and of the reinforcing stimuli, it was demonstrated that implicit operant learning modulated pain sensitivity. All of these aspects of awareness were demonstrated to not be necessary for learning. Additionally, discrimination of contingencies (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26. An operant analysis of problem solving.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):583-591.
    Behavior that solves a problem is distinguished by the fact that it changes another part of the solver's behavior and is strengthened when it does so. Problem solving typically involves the construction of discriminative stimuli. Verbal responses produce especially useful stimuli, because they affect other people. As a culture formulates maxims, laws, grammar, and science, its members behave more effectively without direct or prolonged contact with the contingencies thus formulated. The culture solves problems for its members, and does (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  27. Spirituality, belief, and action.H. W. Reese - 1997 - Journal of Mind and Behavior 18 (1):29-52.
    This article presents an analysis of "spirituality." Ryle said that a belief is not known to be truly held unless one bases crucial action on it; but the qualifications "truly" and "crucial" can be stripped away. Spirituality then becomes consistency of actual action with a belief; or in behavior analytic terms, spirituality is "rule-governed" behavior. Beliefs can function not only as "discriminative stimuli" but also as "reinforcing stimuli." A belief need not correspond to the world as experienced in (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28.  70
    Minding behavior.Peter R. Killeen - 2004 - Behavior and Philosophy 32 (1):125-147.
    There is a conflict of interest in behaviorism between diction and content, between clean speech and effective speech, between what we say and what we know. This article gives a framework for speech that is both clean and effective, that respects graded validation of hypotheses, and that favors distinction over doctrine. The article begins with the description of SDT, a mathematical model of discrimination based on statistical decision theory, which serves as leitmotif. It adopts Skinner's distinction between tacts and mands, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29.  30
    More on the Goodness of Skinner.George Graham - 1983 - Behavior and Philosophy 11 (1):45.
    Discusses B. F. Skinner's proposal in Beyond Freedom and Dignity that reinforcing stimuli are important in the production and modification of value talk. The argument that the view that values are reinforcing leads to moral nihilism is discussed. It is concluded that moral standards can be objective without being universally deployable, and that Skinnerian morality is objective. It shows that certain actions are morally appropriate, others morally wrong. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved).
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  29
    Differential Resistance to Extinction Determined by a Small Number of Differential Instrumental Conditioning Trials.James R. Ison & Allen A. Adinolfi - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (2):350.
  31.  41
    The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  32.  11
    Stochasticity, Nonlinear Value Functions, and Update Rules in Learning Aesthetic Biases.Norberto M. Grzywacz - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15:639081.
    A theoretical framework for the reinforcement learning of aesthetic biases was recently proposed based on brain circuitries revealed by neuroimaging. A model grounded on that framework accounted for interesting features of human aesthetic biases. These features included individuality, cultural predispositions, stochastic dynamics of learning and aesthetic biases, and the peak-shift effect. However, despite the success in explaining these features, a potential weakness was the linearity of the value function used to predict reward. This linearity meant that the learning process employed (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  22
    Comparison of two methods for producing response inhibition in electrodermal conditioning.William W. Grings, Cheryl A. Carey & Anne M. Schell - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (4):658.
  34. The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience.Vilayanur Ramachandran & William Hirstein - 1999 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 6 (6-7):15-41.
    We present a theory of human artistic experience and the neural mechanisms that mediate it. Any theory of art has to ideally have three components. The logic of art: whether there are universal rules or principles; The evolutionary rationale: why did these rules evolve and why do they have the form that they do; What is the brain circuitry involved? Our paper begins with a quest for artistic universals and proposes a list of ‘Eight laws of artistic experience’ -- a (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   101 citations  
  35. The operational analysis of psychological terms.B. F. Skinner - 1945 - Psychological Review 52 (4):270-78.
    The major contributions of operationism have been negative, largely because operationists failed to distinguish logical theories of reference from empirical accounts of language. Behaviorism never finished an adequate formulation of verbal reports and therefore could not convincingly embrace subjective terms. But verbal responses to private stimuli can arise as social products through the contingencies of reinforcement arranged by verbal communities.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   105 citations  
  36.  48
    Learned material content and acquisition level modulate cerebral reactivation during posttraining rapid-eye-movements sleep.Axel Cleeremans - unknown
    We have previously shown that several brain areas are activated both during sequence learning at wake and during subsequent rapid-eye-movements (REM) sleep (Nat. Neurosci. 3 (2000) 831– 836), suggesting that REM sleep participates in the reprocessing of recent memory traces in humans. However, the nature of the reprocessed information remains open. Here, we show that regional cerebral reactivation during posttraining REM sleep is not merely related to the acquisition of basic visuomotor skills during prior practice of the serial reaction time (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37. The Transition to Experiencing: II. The Evolution of Associative Learning Based on Feelings.Simona Ginsburg & Eva Jablonka - 2007 - Biological Theory 2 (3):231-243.
    We discuss the evolutionary transition from animals with limited experiencing to animals with unlimited experiencing and basic consciousness. This transition was, we suggest, intimately linked with the evolution of associative learning and with flexible reward systems based on, and modifiable by, learning. During associative learning, new pathways relating stimuli and effects are formed within a highly integrated and continuously active nervous system. We argue that the memory traces left by such new stimulus-effect relations form dynamic, flexible, and varied global (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  38. Apparent brightness enhancement in the Kanizsa square with and without illusory contours.Birgitta Dresp & Jean Lorenceau - 1990 - Perception 19:483-489.
    The perceived strength of darkness enhancement in the centre of surfaces surrounded or not surrounded by illusory contours was investigated as a function of proximity of the constituent elements of the display and their angular size. Magnitude estimation was used to measure the perception of the darkness phenomenon in white-on-grey stimuli. Darkness enhancement was perceived in both types of the stimuli used, but more strongly in the presence of illusory contours. In both cases, perceived darkness enhancement increased with (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  39. What are Emotional States, and Why Do We Have Them?Edmund T. Rolls - 2013 - Emotion Review 5 (3):241-247.
    An approach to emotion is described in which emotions are defined as states elicited by instrumental reinforcers, that is, by stimuli that are the goals for action. This leads to a theory of the evolutionary adaptive value of emotions, which is that different genes specify different goals in their own self-interest, and any actions can then be learned and performed by instrumental learning to obtain the goals. The brain mechanisms for emotion in brain regions such as the orbitofrontal and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  40.  59
    Unconscious manipulation of free choice in humans.Andrea Kiesel, Annika Wagener, Wilfried Kunde, Joachim Hoffmann, Andreas J. Fallgatter & Christian Stöcker - 2006 - Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2):397-408.
    Previous research has shown that subliminally presented stimuli accelerate or delay responses afforded by supraliminally presented stimuli. Our experiments extend these findings by showing that unconscious stimuli even affect free choices between responses. Thus, actions that are phenomenally experienced as freely chosen are influenced without the actor becoming aware of the manipulation. However, the unconscious influence is limited to a response bias, as participants chose the primed response only in up to 60% of the trials. LRP data (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  41.  20
    Are self-deceivers enhancing positive affect or denying negative affect? Toward an understanding of implicit affective processes.Michael D. Robinson, Sara K. Moeller & Paul W. Goetz - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (1):152-180.
    Self-deception is an important construct in social, personality, and clinical literatures. Although historical and clinical views of self-deception have regarded it as defensive in nature and operation, modern views of this individual difference variable instead highlight its apparent benefits to subjective mental health. The present four studies reinforce the latter view by showing that self-deception predicts positive priming effects, but not negative priming effects, in reaction time tasks sensitive to individual differences in affective priming. In all studies, individuals higher in (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  42.  17
    Differential aversive learning enhances orientation discrimination.L. Jack Rhodes, Aholibama Ruiz, Matthew Ríos, Thomas Nguyen & Vladimir Miskovic - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (4):885-891.
    A number of recent studies have documented rapid changes in behavioural sensory acuity induced by aversive learning in the olfactory and auditory modalities. The effect of aversive learning on the discrimination of low-level features in the visual system of humans remains unclear. Here, we used a psychophysical staircase procedure to estimate discrimination thresholds for oriented grating stimuli, before and after differential aversive learning. We discovered that when a target grating orientation was conditioned with an aversive loud noise, it subsequently (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  16
    Learned helplessness and its relevance for psychological suffering: a new perspective illustrated with attachment problems, burn-out, and fatigue complaints.Yannick Boddez, Pieter Van Dessel & Jan De Houwer - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (6):1027-1036.
    We develop a new perspective on various forms of psychological suffering – including attachment issues, burn-out, and fatigue complaints – by drawing on the construct of learned helplessness. We conceptualise learned helplessness in operant terms as the behavioural effects of a lack of reinforcement and in goal-directed terms as the dysregulation of goal-directed behaviour. Our central claim is that if one fails to reach a goal (e.g. the goal to secure a job), then not only this goal but also other (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44. Pedagogies of Reflection: Dialogical Professional-Development Schools in Israel.Arie Kizel - 2014 - Advances in Research on Teaching 22:113 – 136.
    This chapter discusses a form of pedagogy of reflection suggested to be defined as the dialogical-reflective professional-development school (DRPDS)  a framework that develops and empowers students by engaging them in a process of continual improvement, responding to diverse situations, providing stimuli for learning, and giving anchors for mediation. The pedagogy of reflection relates to dialogue not only from a theoretical historical context but also by way of example  that is, it offers empowering dialogues within the traditional teacher-training (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  39
    The dance form of the eyes: what cognitive science can learn from art.Ralph D. Ellis - 1999 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 6 (6-7):6-7.
    Art perception offers action affordances for the self-generated movement of the eyes, the mind, and the emotions; thus some scenes are ’easy to look at', and evoke different kinds of moods depending on what kind of affordances they present for the eyes, the brain, and the action schemas that further the dynamical self-organizing patterns of activity toward which the organism tends, as reflected in its ongoing emotional life. Art can do this only because perception is active rather than passive, and (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  46.  23
    Affect systems and neural systems.Eric A. Salzen - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (2):216-217.
    The “reward” systems described by Rolls are systems for drive-reinforced associations of contact and distant stimuli and not for emotional behaviours. The neural systems delineated may be associated with distinct categories of “affect,” namely “hedonic feelings,” “moods,” and “emotions.” Awareness of these affects requires external perceptual as well as internal feedback. Levels of feedback in evolution and development suggest sensory qualia may not require language.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  31
    Preparing to caress: a neural signature of social bonding.Rafaela R. Campagnoli, Laura Krutman, Claudia D. Vargas, Isabela Lobo, Jose M. Oliveira, Leticia Oliveira, Mirtes G. Pereira, Isabel A. David & Eliane Volchan - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:121308.
    It is assumed that social bonds in humans have consequences for virtually all aspects of behavior. Social touch-based contact, particularly hand caressing, plays an important role in social bonding. Pre-programmed neural circuits likely support actions (or predispositions to act) towards caressing contacts. We searched for pre-set motor substrates towards caressing by exposing volunteers to bonding cues and having them gently stroke a very soft cloth, a caress-like movement. The bonding cues were pictures with interacting dyads and the control pictures presented (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  48.  43
    Keep focussing: striatal dopamine multiple functions resolved in a single mechanism tested in a simulated humanoid robot.Vincenzo G. Fiore, Valerio Sperati, Francesco Mannella, Marco Mirolli, Kevin Gurney, Karl Friston, Raymond J. Dolan & Gianluca Baldassarre - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    The effects of striatal dopamine (DA) on behavior have been widely investigated over the past decades, with “phasic” burst firings considered as the key expression of a reward prediction error responsible for reinforcement learning. Less well studied is “tonic” DA, where putative functions include the idea that it is a regulator of vigor, incentive salience, disposition to exert an effort and a modulator of approach strategies. We present a model combining tonic and phasic DA to show how different outflows triggered (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  49.  12
    Concerning Ritual Practice and Ethics in Buddhism.Donald W. Mitchell - 2000 - Buddhist-Christian Studies 20 (1):84-89.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Buddhist-Christian Studies 20 (2000) 84-89 [Access article in PDF] Christian Views on Ritual Practice Concerning Ritual Practice and Ethics in Buddhism Donald W. MitchellPurdue UniversityThe three papers presented by this panel have given me a much greater knowledge about, and appreciation for, the relationship between ritual practice and ethical action in Tibetan, Zen, and Nichiren Buddhism. I would like to respond to each of the papers one at a (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  34
    John Paul II and Interreligious Dialogue (review).Donald W. Mitchell - 2000 - Buddhist-Christian Studies 20 (1):303-311.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Buddhist-Christian Studies 20 (2000) 84-89 [Access article in PDF] Christian Views on Ritual Practice Concerning Ritual Practice and Ethics in Buddhism Donald W. MitchellPurdue UniversityThe three papers presented by this panel have given me a much greater knowledge about, and appreciation for, the relationship between ritual practice and ethical action in Tibetan, Zen, and Nichiren Buddhism. I would like to respond to each of the papers one at a (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 1000