Results for 'Understanding goal‐directed action'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Generality and perceptual constraints in understanding goal-directed actions in young infants.I. Király, B. Jovanovic, G. Aschersleben, W. Prinz & G. Gergely - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (4):752-769.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  12
    What are the consequences of understanding the complex goal-directed actions of others?Mary Gauvain - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (5):700-701.
    Four issues that build on the ideas offered by Tomasello et al. are discussed: the developmental course of shared intentionality and its relation to other developing abilities and experiences, and the conceptualization of three key features of the process: motivation, plans and the development of planning, and culture.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  17
    Editorial: Infants' Understanding and Production of Goal-Directed Actions in the Context of Social and Object-Related Interactions.Daniela Corbetta & Jacqueline Fagard - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  20
    Directed Action and Animal Communication.Daisie Radner - 1993 - Ratio 6 (2):135-154.
    Human action theory, with its emphasis on intentions and reasons, does little to enhance our understanding of the actions of nonhuman animals. Many animal (and human) actions are directed to objects in the world, including other animals. The notion of directedness can be analysed without attributing intentions or reasons to the agent. An action is directed to object X if and only if: (1) the agent singles out X, either by orientation or by selective performance of the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  5. Supervision, presence and knowledge: clarifying ‘parental monitoring’ concepts within a model of goal-directed parental action.Beth Hardie - forthcoming - Theory and Society:1-27.
    The presence of parents or other guardians (commonly termed ‘supervision’) and parental knowledge are factors that are both robustly negatively associated with a range of anti-social and risky behavioural outcomes such as adolescent crime. However, parental presence/supervision and parental knowledge are both (i) regularly used inaccurately as proxies for parental monitoring, (ii) poorly defined and operationalised, and (iii) rarely linked to negative behavioural outcomes with plausible mechanisms that adequately explain their association. These problematic aspects of the parental monitoring literature are (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  32
    Directed action and animal communication.Daisie Radner - 1993 - Ration 6 (2):135-54.
    Human action theory, with its emphasis on intentions and reasons, does little to enhance our understanding of the actions of nonhuman animals. Many animal (and human) actions are directed to objects in the world, including other animals. The notion of directedness can be analysed without attributing intentions or reasons to the agent. An action is directed to object X if and only if: (1) the agent singles out X, either by orientation or by selective performance of the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  7. Environmentally Sustainable Food Consumption: A Review and Research Agenda From a Goal-Directed Perspective.Iris Vermeir, Bert Weijters, Jan De Houwer, Maggie Geuens, Hendrik Slabbinck, Adriaan Spruyt, Anneleen Van Kerckhove, Wendy Van Lippevelde, Hans De Steur & Wim Verbeke - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    The challenge of convincing people to change their eating habits towards more environmentally sustainable food consumption (ESFC) patterns is becoming increasingly pressing. Food preferences, choices and eating habits are notoriously hard to change as they are a central aspect of people’s lifestyles and their socio-cultural environment. Many people already hold positive attitudes towards sustainable food, but the notable gap between favorable attitudes and actual purchase and consumption of more sustainable food products remains to be bridged. The current work aims to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  8.  8
    Learned helplessness revisited: biased evaluation of goals and action potential are major risk factors for emotional disturbance.Klaus R. Scherer - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (6):1021-1026.
    The present theory section deals with learned helplessness produced by pervasive experiences of failure or negative events, leading to decreased motivation and risk for depression. In their target article, Boddez, van Dessel, and de Houwer apply this concept to different forms of psychological suffering and propose a goal-directed mechanism –generalisation over similar goals. Duda and Joormann define goal similarity by action-outcome contingencies and highlight individual differences in attribution styles. Brandstätter proposes incentive classes as the organising principle for goal similarity (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  9
    E very day, from the time we wake up in the morning until the time we go to bed, goals influence our thoughts, feelings, and actions. For instance, our.Basic Goal Distinctions - 2012 - In Henk Aarts & Andrew J. Elliot (eds.), Goal-directed behavior. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. Goal-Directed Action: Teleological Explanations, Causal Theories, and Deviance.Alfred R. Mele - 2000 - Noûs 34 (s14):279 - 300.
    Teleological explanations of human actions are explanations in terms of aims, goals, or purposes of human agents. According to a familiar causal approach to analyzing and explaining human action, our actions are, essentially, events (and sometimes states, perhaps) that are suitably caused by appropriate mental items, or neural realizations of those items. Causalists traditionally appeal, in part, to such goal-representing states as desires and intentions (or their neural realizers) in their explanations of human actions, and they take accept-able teleological (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  11.  2
    Goal‐Directed Action.Alfred R. Mele - 2003 - In Motivation and agency. New York: Oxford University Press.
    Argues for a constraint on a proper theory of motivation – namely, that proper motivational explanations of goal‐directed actions are causal explanations. The chapter criticizes the thesis that acceptable teleological explanations of actions are not causal explanations and it offers a solution to a problem that deviant causal chains pose for a causal theory of action.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. Goal-directed Action and Eligible Forms of Embodiment.D. Vernon - 2013 - Constructivist Foundations 9 (1):85-85.
    Open peer commentary on the article “Investigating Extended Embodiment Using a Computational Model and Human Experimentation” by Yuki Sato, Hiroyuki Iizuka & Takashi Ikegami. Upshot: The target article’s findings on the focus of attention in extending an agent’s body schema are consistent with those in developmental psychology and neuroscience on goal-directed action. The consequences of these findings are that embodiment can be extended in a variety of ways, not all of which require direct physical manipulation.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  6
    Goal-directed action in hemispatial neglect.Marlene Behrmann & Daniel V. Meegan - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):381-409.
  14.  64
    8 Goal-Directed Action and Teleological Explanation.Scott R. Sehon - 2007 - In J. K. Campbell, M. O'Rourke & H. S. Silverstein (eds.), Causation and Explanation. MIT Press. pp. 4--155.
  15.  19
    The Politics of Legal Abortion: From Direct Action to Dialogue.Jeffrey A. Gauthier - 2021 - Hypatia 36 (4):800-804.
    In her highly influential 1984 study Abortion and the Politics of Motherhood, Kristin Luker speculates that opposition to legal abortion among women was likely to be strongest among those who were full-time homemakers without a college education. But despite a marked decline in that demographic group and a well-documented rise in public support for gender equality since then, the rate of support for legal abortion has remained stubbornly fixed at between fifty and fifty-five percent. This tepid support has coincided with (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16. The organization of goal-directed action: A research report.M. Von Cranach, Elfie Machler & Vera Steiner - 1985 - In G. P. Ginsburg, Marylin Brenner & Mario von Cranach (eds.), Discovery Strategies in the Psychology of Action. Academic Press. pp. 19--61.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  42
    Newborns’ preference for goal-directed actions.Laila Craighero, Irene Leo, Carlo Umiltà & Francesca Simion - 2011 - Cognition 120 (1):26-32.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  18.  58
    From discrete actors to goal-directed actions: Toward a process-based methodology for psychology.Endre E. Kadar & Judith A. Effken - 2005 - Philosophical Psychology 18 (3):353 – 382.
    Studying social phenomena is often assumed to be inherently different from studying natural science phenomena. In psychology, this assumption has led to a division of the field into social and experimental domains. The same kind of division has carried over into ecological psychology, despite the fact that Gibson clearly intended his theory for both social and natural phenomena. In this paper, we argue that the social/natural science dichotomy can be derived from a distinction between hermeneutics and science that is deeply (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  19.  18
    Input-output relations in goal-directed actions.M. Jeannerod - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):628-629.
  20.  24
    Action Understanding in Infancy: Do Infant Interpreters Attribute Enduring Mental States or Track Relational Properties of Transient Bouts of Behavior?Marco Fenici & Tadeusz Zawidzki - 2016 - Studia Philosophica Estonica 9 (1):237-257.
    We address recent interpretations of infant performance on spontaneous false belief tasks. According to most views, these experiments show that human infants attribute mental states from a very young age. Focusing on one of the most clearly worked out, minimalist versions of this idea, Butterfill and Apperly's "minimal theory of mind" framework, we defend an alternative characterization: the minimal theory of rational agency. On this view, rather than conceiving of social situations in terms of states of an enduring mental substance (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  21.  72
    Goals are not implied by actions, but inferred from actions and contexts.Iris van Rooij, Willem Haselager & Harold Bekkering - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (1):38-39.
    People cannot understand intentions behind observed actions by direct simulation, because goal inference is highly context dependent. Context dependency is a major source of computational intractability in traditional information-processing models. An embodied embedded view of cognition may be able to overcome this problem, but then the problem needs recognition and explication within the context of the new, layered cognitive architecture.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  29
    The Weight of Emotions in Decision-Making: How Fearful and Happy Facial Stimuli Modulate Action Readiness of Goal-Directed Actions.Giovanni Mirabella - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  23.  26
    Goal directed behavior: the concept of action in psychology.Michael Frese & John Sabini (eds.) - 1985 - Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates.
  24.  11
    Dopaminergic modulation of positive expectations for goal-directed action: evidence from Parkinson’s disease.Noham Wolpe, Cristina Nombela & James B. Rowe - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  25.  26
    The Phenomenology of Eye Movement Intentions and their Disruption in Goal-Directed Actions.Maximilian Roszko, Lars Hall, Petter Johansson & Philip Pärnamets - 2018 - In Timothy M. Rogers, Marina Rau, Jerry Zhu & Chuck Kalish (eds.), Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 973-978.
    The role of intentions in motor planning is heavily weighted in classical psychological theories, but their role in generating eye movements, and our awareness of these oculomotor intentions, has not been investigated explicitly. In this study, the extent to which we monitor oculomotor intentions, i.e. the intentions to shift one’s gaze towards a specific location, and whether they can be expressed in conscious experience, is investigated. A forced-choice decision task was developed where a pair of faces moved systematically across a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  47
    The Power of Goal-Directed Processes in the Causation of Emotional and Other Actions.Agnes Moors, Yannick Boddez & Jan De Houwer - 2017 - Emotion Review 9 (4):310-318.
    Standard dual-process models in the action domain postulate that stimulus-driven processes are responsible for suboptimal behavior because they take them to be rigid and automatic and therefore the default. We propose an alternative dual-process model in which goal-directed processes are the default instead. We then transfer the dual- process logic from the action domain to the emotion domain. This reveals that emotional behavior is often attributed to stimulus-driven processes. Our alternative model submits that goal-directed processes could be the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  27.  11
    Explicit Sense of Agency in an Automatic Control Situation: Effects of Goal-Directed Action and the Gradual Emergence of Outcome.Ryoichi Nakashima & Takatsune Kumada - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition.Michael Tomasello, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne & Henrike Moll - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (5):675-691.
    We propose that the crucial difference between human cognition and that of other species is the ability to participate with others in collaborative activities with shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality. Participation in such activities requires not only especially powerful forms of intention reading and cultural learning, but also a unique motivation to share psychological states with others and unique forms of cognitive representation for doing so. The result of participating in these activities is species-unique forms of cultural cognition and (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   510 citations  
  29.  11
    To Train or to Entertain the Brain: How Does Enhanced Focus of Attention Guide Perception into the Goal Directed Action.Gorjup Rado & Gorjup Niko - 2017 - Philosophy Study 7 (8).
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  22
    Social meaning in the observation of goal directed action.Ladislav Valach, Mario von Cranach & Urs Kalbermatten - 1988 - Semiotica 71 (3-4):243-260.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  56
    Intentional action processing results from automatic bottom-up attention: An EEG-investigation into the Social Relevance Hypothesis using hypnosis.Eleonore Neufeld, Elliot C. Brown, Sie-In Lee-Grimm, Albert Newen & Martin Brüne - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 42:101-112.
    Social stimuli grab our attention: we attend to them in an automatic and bottom-up manner, and ascribe them a higher degree of saliency compared to non-social stimuli. However, it has rarely been investigated how variations in attention affect the processing of social stimuli, although the answer could help us uncover details of social cognition processes such as action understanding. In the present study, we examined how changes to bottom-up attention affects neural EEG-responses associated with intentional action processing. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  32.  37
    Foundations of Understanding.Natika Newton - 1996 - John Benjamins.
    How can symbols have meaning for a subject? Foundations of Understanding argues that this is the key question to ask about intentionality, or meaningful thought. It thus offers an alternative to currently popular linguistic models of intentionality, whose inadequacies are examined: the goal should be to explain, not how symbols, mental or otherwise, can refer to or mean states of affairs in the external world, but how they can mean something to us, the users. The essence of intentionality is (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  33. The influence of goal-directed movements on ideomotor action.S. De Maeght, L. Knuf & W. Prinz - 2000 - Consciousness and Cognition 9 (2):S77 - S78.
  34. Action is goal-directed.Michael Brenner - 1985 - In G. P. Ginsburg, Marylin Brenner & Mario von Cranach (eds.), Discovery Strategies in the Psychology of Action. Academic Press. pp. 35--207.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  22
    Infants' understanding of object-directed action.Ann T. Phillips & Henry M. Wellman - 2005 - Cognition 98 (2):137-155.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  36.  21
    Content-based control of goal-directed attention during human action perception.Yiannis Demiris & Bassam Khadhouri - 2008 - Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies / Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies 9 (2):353-376.
    During the perception of human actions by robotic assistants, the robotic assistant needs to direct its computational and sensor resources to relevant parts of the human action. In previous work we have introduced HAMMER, a computational architecture that forms multiple hypotheses with respect to what the demonstrated task is, and multiple predictions with respect to the forthcoming states of the human action. To confirm their predictions, the hypotheses request information from an attentional mechanism, which allocates the robot’s resources (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  17
    Infants’ Understanding of Object-Directed Action: An Interdisciplinary Synthesis.Scott J. Robson & Valerie A. Kuhlmeier - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  38. Are visuomotor representations cognitively penetrable? Biasing action-guiding vision.Josefa Toribio - 2018 - Synthese (Suppl 17):1-19.
    Is action-guiding vision cognitively penetrable? More specifically, is the visual processing that guides our goal-directed actions sensitive to semantic information from cognitive states? This paper critically examines a recent family of arguments whose aim is to challenge a widespread and influential view in philosophy and cognitive science: the view that action-guiding vision is cognitively impenetrable. I argue, in response, that while there may very well be top–down causal influences on action-guiding vision, they should not be taken to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  39. Habitual Virtuous Actions and Automaticity.Nancy E. Snow - 2006 - Ethical Theory and Moral Practice 9 (5):545-561.
    Dual process theorists in psychology maintain that the mind’s workings can be explained in terms of conscious or controlled processes and automatic processes. Automatic processes are largely nonconscious, that is, triggered by environmental stimuli without the agent’s conscious awareness or deliberation. Automaticity researchers contend that even higher level habitual social behaviors can be nonconsciously primed. This article brings work on automaticity to bear on our understanding of habitual virtuous actions. After examining a recent intuitive account of habitual actions and (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  40.  52
    Primacy of Information About Means Selection Over Outcome Selection in Goal Attribution by Infants.Stephan Verschoor & Szilvia Biro - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (4):714-725.
    It has been shown that, when observing an action, infants can rely on either outcome selection information (i.e., actions that express a choice between potential outcomes) or means selection information (i.e., actions that are causally efficient toward the outcome) in their goal attribution. However, no research has investigated the relationship between these two types of information when they are present simultaneously. In an experiment that addressed this question directly, we found that when outcome selection information could disambiguate the goal (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  41. The Role of Intention in Intentional Action.Frederick Adams & Alfred Mele - 1989 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 19 (4):511 - 531.
    A great deal of attention has been paid in recent years to the function- al roles of intentions in intentional action. In this paper we sketch and defend a position on this issue while attacking a provocative alternative. Our position has its roots in a cybernetic theory of purposive behavior and is only part of the larger task of understanding all goal-directed behavior. Indeed, a unified model of goal-directed behavior, with appropriate modifications for different types of systems, is (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  42.  30
    Understanding intentions from actions: Direct perception, inference, and the roles of mirror and mentalizing systems.Caroline Catmur - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 36:426-433.
  43.  13
    Similar Mechanisms of Movement Control in Target- and Effect-Directed Actions toward Spatial Goals?Andrea M. Walter & Martina Rieger - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44. Learning of New Percept-Action Mappings Is a Constructive Process of Goal-Directed Self-Modification.P. A. Cariani - 2016 - Constructivist Foundations 11 (2):322-324.
    Open peer commentary on the article “Perception-Action Mutuality Obviates Mental Construction” by Martin Flament Fultot, Lin Nie & Claudia Carello. Upshot: In my view, the clash between ecological psychology, enactivism, and constructivism in general has more to do with irreconcilable metaphysical and theoretical incommensurabilities than disagreements about specific mechanisms or processes of perception. Even with mutual enabling of action and perception, some internal process of self-modification is still needed if novel behavior is to be adequately explained.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  45.  36
    Understanding Feasibility of Climate Change Goals and Actions.Anna Döhlen Wedin - 2024 - Ethics, Policy and Environment 27 (1):48-62.
    Climate change goals and actions are often discussed with reference to their feasibility. However, in the climate change literature, there is no agreed upon understanding of what feasibility means. In this paper, insights from political philosophy are used to address this problem in a two-fold way. First, different uses of the term feasibility in the climate change context are critically analyzed, surfacing problematic uses that can have severe consequences for what goals or actions are considered. Second, the ‘conditional probability (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  48
    A second-person approach cannot explain intentionality in social understanding.Chris Moore & Markus Paulus - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (4):430-431.
    A second-person approach that prioritizes dyadic emotional interaction is not well equipped to explain the origins of the understanding of mind conceived as intentionality. Instead, the critical elements that will deliver the understanding of self and other as persons with intentionality are shared object-centered interactions that include not only emotional engagement, but also joint attention and joint goal-directed action.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  47. Goal directed behavior as a source of stress: Psychological origins and consequences of inefficiency.Wolfgang Schönpflug - 1985 - In Michael Frese & John Sabini (eds.), Goal Directed Behavior: The Concept of Action in Psychology. L. Erlbaum Associates. pp. 172--188.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  57
    Direct Action and the Climate Crisis.Reed M. Kurtz - 2020 - Radical Philosophy Review 23 (2):261-297.
    How should we conceptualize direct action against climate change? Although direct action is an increasingly significant tactic by the global climate movement, we lack understanding how direct action contributes to the systemic change necessary for addressing the crisis. Drawing upon critical theories of climate change as a crisis in the social reproduction of the metabolic relations between humans and nature in capitalism, I conceptualize direct action as attempts to intervene directly in the organization of the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  19
    Understanding Consciousness Using Systems Approaches and Lexical Universals.Michael Winkelman - 2004 - Anthropology of Consciousness 15 (2):24-38.
    The numerous perspectives offered on consciousness reflect a multifaceted phenomenon that results from a system of relations. An etymological approach identifies linguistic roots of the meanings of consciousness and illustrates their concern with self-referenced informational relationships of an organism with its environment, a "knowing system" formed in the epistemological relations between knower and known. Common elements of contemporary models suggest that consciousness involves interacting components of a system, including: attention-awareness; phenomenal experiences; self reference; action-behavior, including representations and learning; use (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  50.  28
    The Feeling of Action Tendencies: On the Emotional Regulation of Goal-Directed Behavior.Robert Lowe & Tom Ziemke - 2011 - Frontiers in Psychology 2.
1 — 50 / 1000