Results for ' all-or-none learning'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  3
    All-or-none learning of attributes.Albert S. Bregman & David W. Chambers - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (6):785.
  2.  8
    Stimulus emphasis and all-or-none learning in concept identification.Thomas R. Trabasso - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (4):398.
  3.  3
    Nonstationary performance before all-or-none learning.Peter G. Polson & James G. Greeno - 1969 - Psychological Review 76 (2):227-231.
  4.  5
    All-or-none and conservation effects in the learning and retention of paired associates.W. K. Estes, B. L. Hopkins & E. J. Crothers - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (6):329.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  1
    Associative and differentiation variables in all-or-none learning.Clessen J. Martin - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (3):308.
  6.  8
    All-or-none versus incremental learning.Joan E. Jones - 1962 - Psychological Review 69 (2):156-160.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  6
    An all-or-none characteristic in the elimination of errors during the learning of a stylus maze.J. A. McGeoch & H. N. Peters - 1933 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 16 (4):504.
  8.  5
    A test of the all-or-none hypothesis for verbal learning.Joanna P. Williams - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (2):158.
  9.  1
    Simple conditioning as two-stage all-or-none learning.John Theios - 1963 - Psychological Review 70 (5):403-417.
  10.  7
    All-or-none assumptions in concept identification: Analysis of latency data.James R. Erickson, Myron M. Zajkowski & Evan D. Ehmann - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (5):690.
  11.  7
    Forgetting in short-term recall: All-or-none or decremental?Thomas O. Nelson & William H. Batchelder - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (1p1):96.
  12.  24
    Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach.Richard W. Byrne & Anne E. Russon - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (5):667-684.
    To explain social learning without invoking the cognitively complex concept of imitation, many learning mechanisms have been proposed. Borrowing an idea used routinely in cognitive psychology, we argue that most of these alternatives can be subsumed under a single process, priming, in which input increases the activation of stored internal representations. Imitation itself has generally been seen as a This has diverted much research towards the all-or-none question of whether an animal can imitate, with disappointingly inconclusive results. (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  13.  14
    Optimal problem-solving search: All-or-none solutions.Herbert A. Simon & Joseph B. Kadane - 1975 - Artificial Intelligence 6 (3):235-247.
  14.  3
    All or None: a Novel Choice of Primitives for Elementary Logic.R. H. Thomason & H. Leblanc - 1969 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 34 (1):124-125.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15.  1
    All or none; A novel choice of primitives for elementary logic.R. H. Thomason & H. Leblanc - 1967 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 32 (3):345-351.
  16. All-or-none versus a graded process conception of attention.L. R. Fournier & C. W. Eriksen - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (6):518-518.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  10
    Consciousness isn’t all-or-none: Evidence for partial awareness during the attentional blink.James C. Elliott, Benjamin Baird & Barry Giesbrecht - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 40:79-85.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  18.  4
    Tests of an all-or-none model of verbal mediated responding.Kent L. Norman & Irwin P. Levin - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 96 (2):247.
  19.  6
    A fundamental property of all-or-none models, binomial distribution of responses prior to conditioning, with application to concept formation in children.Patrick Suppes & Rose Ginsberg - 1963 - Psychological Review 70 (2):139-161.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  20.  10
    Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink.Claire Sergent & Stanislas Dehaene - 2004 - Psychological Science 15 (11):720-728.
  21.  16
    Representational systems.Tomer Fekete - 2010 - Minds and Machines 20 (1):69-101.
    The concept of representation has been a key element in the scientific study of mental processes, ever since such studies commenced. However, usage of the term has been all but too liberal—if one were to adhere to common use it remains unclear if there are examples of physical systems which cannot be construed in terms of representation. The problem is considered afresh, taking as the starting point the notion of activity spaces—spaces of spatiotemporal events produced by dynamical systems. It is (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  22.  10
    Consciousness as a graded and an all-or-none phenomenon: A conceptual analysis.Bert Windey & Axel Cleeremans - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 35:185-191.
  23.  9
    The Competence of Children: No Longer All or None.Willard Gaylin - 1982 - Hastings Center Report 12 (2):33-38.
  24. Paradox Revisited II: Sets—A Case of All or None.Hilary Putnam - 2000 - In Gila Sher & Richard Tieszen (eds.), Between logic and intuition: essays in honor of Charles Parsons. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 16--26.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  25.  8
    R. H. Thomason and H. Leblanc. All or none: a novel choice of primitives for elementary logic. The journal of symbolic logic, vol. 32 , pp. 345–351.Mitsuru Yasuhara - 1969 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 34 (1):124-125.
  26.  7
    Controlling the urge for a Ca2+ surge: all‐or‐none Ca2+ release in neurons.Yuriy M. Usachev & Stanley A. Thayer - 1999 - Bioessays 21 (9):743-750.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  3
    If priming is graded rather than all-or-none, can reactivating abstract structures be the underlying mechanism?Laurie Beth Feldman & Petar Milin - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28. The delayed consolidation hypothesis of all-or-none conscious perception during the attentional blink, applying the ST2 framework.H. Bowman, Patrick Craston, Srivas Chennu & Brad Wyble - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  12
    Rachel Pevtzow.Stevan Harnad - unknown
    In innate Categorical Perception (CP) (e.g., colour perception), similarity space is "warped," with regions of increased within-category similarity (compression) and regions of reduced between-category similarity (separation) enh ancing the category boundaries and making categorisation reliable and all-or-none rather than graded. We show that category learning can likewise warp similarity space, resolving uncertainty near category boundaries. Two Hard and two Easy texture learning tasks were compared: As predicted, there were fewer successful Learners with the Hard task, and only (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  6
    If Students Don’t Feel it, They Won’t Learn it: Early Career Secondary Social Studies Educators Plan for Emotional Engagement.Michelle Reidel & Cinthia Salinas - 2024 - Journal of Social Studies Research 48 (2):87-101.
    This qualitative case study examines early career social studies educators’ knowledge of the role of emotion in teaching and learning. More specifically, we examine how our efforts to expand social studies educators’ understanding of emotion, shifted their perception of the role of emotion in learning social studies content and how they can use this knowledge to plan instruction. Prior to beginning their “emotion education,” all participants described the role of emotion in teaching and learning as important for (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  12
    Food Ethics: Paul Pojman , 2011, Wadsworth/cengage Learning.Ben Mepham - 2013 - Journal of Bioethical Inquiry 10 (2):249-251.
    None of us can avoid being interested in food. Our very existence depends on the supply of safe, nutritious foods. It is then hardly surprising that food has become the focus of a wide range of ethical concerns: Is the food we buy safe? Is it produced by means which respect the welfare of animals and sustain the land? Are modern biotechnologies employed in food production immoral? This book addresses such issues by applying ethical principles to many areas of (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  32. A plethora of promises — or none at all.Michael Cholbi - 2014 - American Philosophical Quarterly 51 (3):261-272.
    Utilitarians are supposed to have difficulty accounting for our obligation to keep promises. But utilitarians also face difficulties concerning our obligation to make promises. Consider any situation in which the options available to me are acts A, B, C… n, and A is utility maximizing. Call A+ the course of action consisting of A plus my promising to perform A. Since there appear to be a wide range of instances in which A+ has greater net utility then A, utilitarianism obligates (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  33.  2
    Review: R. H. Thomason, H. Leblanc, All or None: a Novel Choice of Primitives for Elementary Logic. [REVIEW]Mitsuru Yasuhara - 1969 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 34 (1):124-125.
  34.  4
    Too Many Friends or None at All? A “Difference” Between Aristotle and Postmodernity.James McEvoy - 2003 - American Catholic Philosophical Quarterly 77 (1):1-19.
    Diogenes Laertius preserved a saying of Aristotle, “He who has friends can have no true friend.” This was mistranslated by Erasmus and gave rise to the words Montaigne attributed to Aristotle, “O mes amis, il n’y a nul amy.” Kant and Nietzsche both used the saying in this sense, which is in fact a contresens. The original Greek words carried much of the sense of ancient friendship, being a warning against polyphilia and a reminder that intimacy is the central value (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  35. Review of Jesse S. Summers and Walter Sinnott-Armstrong, Clean Hands? Philosophical Lessons from Scrupulosity[REVIEW]Noell Birondo - 2020 - Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews 3.
    Philosophical lessons come in many different shapes and sizes. Some lessons are big, some are small. Some lessons go deep and have a big impact, some are shallow and have almost none. Some lessons are not really philosophical at all or would not really be lessons for an audience of academic philosophers. I mention these truisms not to disparage this informative book on 'moral OCD' (moral obsessive-compulsive disorder, or 'Scrupulosity') but rather to emphasize how difficult it can be to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36.  8
    Does consequentialism make too many demands, or none at all?Paul E. Hurley - 2006 - Ethics 116 (4):680-706.
  37.  6
    Soviet genetics and the communist party: was it all bad and wrong, or none at all?Mikhail Konashev - 2020 - History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 42 (2):1-19.
    The history of genetics and the evolutionary theory in the USSR is multidimensional. Only in the 1920s after the October Revolution, and due in large part to that Revolution, the science of genetics arose in Soviet Russia. Genetics was limited, but not obliterated in the second half of the 1950s, and was restored in the late 1960s, after the resignation of Nikita S. Khrushchev. In the subsequent period, Soviet genetics experienced a resurgence, though one not as successful as geneticists would (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  2
    Killing, asylum, and the law in Byzantium.Ruth J. Macrides - 1988 - Speculum 63 (3):509-538.
    One of the distinguishing characteristics of Byzantium, it is well known, in contrast to the medieval West, is the continuous tradition of Roman law and secular courts which the Eastern Empire possessed throughout its existence, as well as a central authority in a position to put these tools into effect. Thus the question of the nature of law and order in Byzantium would seem to be straightforward; whoever wishes to learn how the crime of killing was handled can consult the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  13
    The none-to-all theorem of human discrimination learning.Marvin Levine, Paul Miller & Charles H. Steinmeyer - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (4p1):568.
  40. Holism, mental and semantic.Ned Block - 1996 - In Edward Craig (ed.), Routledge Encyclopedia of Philosophy: Genealogy to Iqbal. New York: Routledge.
    Mental (or semantic) holism is the doctrine that the identity of a belief content (or the meaning of a sentence that expresses it) is determined by its place in the web of beliefs or sentences comprising a whole theory or group of theories. It can be contrasted with two other views: atomism and molecularism. Molecularism characterizes meaning and content in terms of relatively small parts of the web in a way that allows many different theories to share those parts. For (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  41. Holism, mental and semantic.Ned Block - 1996 - In Edward Craig (ed.), Routledge Encyclopedia of Philosophy: Genealogy to Iqbal. New York: Routledge.
    Mental (or semantic) holism is the doctrine that the identity of a belief content (or the meaning of a sentence that expresses it) is determined by its place in the web of beliefs or sentences comprising a whole theory or group of theories. It can be contrasted with two other views: atomism and molecularism. Molecularism characterizes meaning and content in terms of relatively small parts of the web in a way that allows many different theories to share those parts. For (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42.  6
    We all are rembrandt experts – or, how task dissociations in school learning effects support the discontinuity hypothesis.Régine Kolinsky & José Morais - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (3):381-382.
    We argue that cognitive penetration in non-early vision extends beyond the special situations considered by Pylyshyn. Many situations which do not involve difficult stimuli or require expert skills nevertheless load on high-level cognitive processes. School learning effects illustrate this point: they provide a way to observe task dissociations which support the discontinuity hypothesis, but they show that the scope of visual cognition in our visual experience is often underestimated.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  13
    Teachers or learning leaders?: where have all the teachers gone? gone to be leaders, everyone.Kevin Brain, LouiseComerford Boyes & Ivan Reid * - 2004 - Educational Studies 30 (3):251-264.
    This paper traces the dramatic proliferation of leadership roles in English primary and secondar schools, due mainly to central government education policy of the past two decades. This has transformed schools from relatively simple to highly complex organizations and has impacted on the working conditions of, and demands on, teachers, together with many aspects of schooling. These changes are illustrated with typical examples of schools' leadership structures and their functioning. Interview data provide teachers' views on, and reactions to, the changes (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  44.  4
    “All I really need to know I learned in kindergarten” or is “cultural learning” anthropo-, ethno-, or adultocentric?Heidi Keller & Athanasios Chasiotis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):779-780.
  45.  9
    All the Right Noises: Background Variability Helps Early Word Learning.Katherine E. Twomey, Lizhi Ma & Gert Westermann - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S2):413-438.
    Variability is prevalent in early language acquisition, but, whether it supports or hinders learning is unclear; while target variability has been shown to facilitate word learning, variability in competitor items has been shown to make the task harder. Here, we tested whether background variability could boost learning in a referent selection task. Two groups of 2-year-old children saw arrays of one novel and two known objects on a screen, and they heard a novel or known label. Stimuli (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  46.  3
    Lifelong Learning: Opportunity or Compulsion?Malcom Tight - 1998 - British Journal of Educational Studies 46 (3):251-263.
    Lifelong learning is presented as a means for enabling individuals, organisations and nations to meet the challenges of an increasingly competitive world. It suggests an extension of opportunity,involving all adults, whatever their interests or experience. There is also, however, a strong sense of expectation, even compulsion, with emphasis given to vocational forms of study and participation.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  47.  6
    A question of universality: Inclusive education and the principle of respect.Ruth Cigman - 2007 - Journal of Philosophy of Education 41 (4):775–793.
    The universalist argument that all children should be educated in inclusive mainstream schools, irrespective of their difficulties or disabilities, is traced to the claims that special schools and disability ‘labels’ are inherently humiliating, and that no decent society tolerates inherently humiliating institutions. I ask whether there is a sound reason for a child to feel humiliated by special schools/disability ‘labels’ as such, and find none. Empirically, some do and some do not find these humiliating, and it is argued that (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  48.  3
    A Question of Universality: Inclusive Education and the Principle of Respect.Ruth Cigman - 2007 - Journal of Philosophy of Education 41 (4):775-793.
    The universalist argument that all children should be educated in inclusive mainstream schools, irrespective of their difficulties or disabilities, is traced to the claims (a) that special schools and disability ‘labels’ are inherently humiliating, and (b) that no decent society tolerates inherently humiliating institutions. I ask (following Avishai Margalit) whether there is a sound reason for a child to feel humiliated by special schools/disability ‘labels’ as such, and find none. Empirically, some do and some do not find these humiliating, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  49.  7
    Manipulations of List Type in the DRM Paradigm: A Review of How Structural and Conceptual Similarity Affect False Memory. [REVIEW]Jennifer H. Coane, Dawn M. McBride, Mark J. Huff, Kai Chang, Elizabeth M. Marsh & Kendal A. Smith - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    The use of list-learning paradigms to explore false memory has revealed several critical findings about the contributions of similarity and relatedness in memory phenomena more broadly. Characterizing the nature of “similarity and relatedness” can inform researchers about factors contributing to memory distortions and about the underlying associative and semantic networks that support veridical memory. Similarity can be defined in terms of semantic properties, lexical/associative properties, or structural properties. By manipulating the type of list and its relationship to a non-studied (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  7
    When all is considered: Evaluative learning does not require contingency awareness.Eamon P. Fulcher & Marianne Hammerl - 2001 - Consciousness and Cognition 10 (4):567-573.
    We argue that the effects of evaluative learning may occur (a) without conscious perception of the affective stimuli, (b) without awareness of the stimulus contingencies, and (c) without any awareness that learning has occurred at all. Whether the three experiments reported in our target article provide conclusive evidence for either or any of these assertions is discussed in the commentaries of De Houwer and Field. We respond with the argument that when considered alongside other studies carried out over (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000