Results for 'hypnotic trance'

328 found
Order:
  1.  16
    Differentiation of the hypnotic trance from normal sleep.M. J. Bass - 1931 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 14 (4):382.
  2.  19
    Does the hypnotic trance favor the recall of faint memories?B. Huse - 1930 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 13 (6):519.
  3.  98
    Hypnotic behavior: A social-psychological interpretation of amnesia, analgesia, and “trance logic”.Nicholas P. Spanos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):449-467.
    This paper examines research on three hypnotic phenomena: suggested amnesia, suggested analgesia, and “trance logic.” For each case a social-psychological interpretation of hypnotic behavior as a voluntary response strategy is compared with the traditional special-process view that “good” hypnotic subjects have lost conscious control over suggestion-induced behavior. I conclude that it is inaccurate to describe hypnotically amnesic subjects as unable to recall the material they have been instructed to forget. Although amnesics present themselves as unable to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   128 citations  
  4.  76
    Trance and shamanic cure on the south american continent: Psychopharmacological and neurobiological interpretations.Francois Blanc - 2010 - Anthropology of Consciousness 21 (1):83-105.
    This article examines the neurobiological basis of the healing power attributed to shamanic practices in the Andes and Brazil in light of the pharmacology of neurotransmitters and the new technological explorations of brain functioning. The psychotropic plants used in shamanic psychiatric cures interfere selectively with the intrinsic neuromediators of the brain. Mainly they may alter: (1) the neuroendocrine functioning through the adrenergic system by controlling stressful conditions, (2) the dopaminergic system in incentive learning and emotions incorporation, (3) the serotoninergic system (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  5.  38
    Hypnotic experience and the autism spectrum disorder. A phenomenological investigation.Till Grohmann - 2018 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 17 (5):889-909.
    In recent decades, the focus in autism research progressively expanded. It presently offers extensive material on sensorimotor disturbances as well as on perceptive-cognitive preferences of people with autism. The present article proposes not only a critical interpretation of the common theoretical framework in autism research but also focuses on certain experiences common to some people with autism and which can be appropriately understood by phenomenology. What I will call “hypnotic experiences” in autism are moments in which some individuals withdraw (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  18
    The influence of repetition and disuse upon rate of hypnotization.R. G. Krueger - 1931 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 14 (3):260.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  7.  55
    Intentional action processing results from automatic bottom-up attention: An EEG-investigation into the Social Relevance Hypothesis using hypnosis.Eleonore Neufeld, Elliot C. Brown, Sie-In Lee-Grimm, Albert Newen & Martin Brüne - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 42:101-112.
    Social stimuli grab our attention: we attend to them in an automatic and bottom-up manner, and ascribe them a higher degree of saliency compared to non-social stimuli. However, it has rarely been investigated how variations in attention affect the processing of social stimuli, although the answer could help us uncover details of social cognition processes such as action understanding. In the present study, we examined how changes to bottom-up attention affects neural EEG-responses associated with intentional action processing. We induced an (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  8.  20
    The spectrum of an altered state of consciousness, where information is accessed or abilities realized beyond what is ordinarily possible.Pam Payne - 2012 - Technoetic Arts 10 (2-3):287-295.
    As an artist I am interested in creative states of consciousness and the direct expression of altered states of consciousness in forms such as musical improvisation and the automatic writings and drawings of the Surrealist Artists. I have been investigating a particular spectrum of altered states characterized by an enhanced experience where out-of-the-ordinary information is accessed or an enhanced ability is realized beyond what would ordinarily be possible. Within this realm we would find the ‘peak performance’ state of athletes and (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  6
    La voix du philosophe Laruelle.Gilbert Kieffer - 2017 - Labyrinth: An International Journal for Philosophy, Value Theory and Sociocultural Hermeneutics 19 (2):121-131.
    The Voice of Laruelle, the philosopher What is a voice in the context of the arts and philosophy? In the space of the philosopher's voice, in the complex grammar of his language is played his philosophical timbre, his own space, his particular voice, composed of concepts, articulated by the laws of coherence of the common philosophical language, with hypnotic specificities. These specificities are precisely the fruit of processes formerly called rhetoric, which I call non-hypnotics, one of whose functions is (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  25
    Of Horses, Planks, and Window Sleepers: Stage Hypnotism Meets Reform, 1836–1920. [REVIEW]Fred Nadis - 2001 - Journal of Medical Humanities 22 (3):223-245.
    This paper is a historical study of stage hypnotism from the early nineteenth through the early twentieth centuries. The hypnotists' stage performances over this period reveal cultural tensions related to modernization. Public responses to these shows also indicate the shifting dynamics of reform. When mesmerists first toured the U.S. in the early nineteenth century, the hypnotic trance confirmed popular belief in the ultimate perfectibility of the individual and society. By the late nineteenth century, however, hypnotic shows seemed (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  35
    Hypnosis and will.Irving Kirsch & Steven Jay Lynn - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (5):667-668.
    Although we are sympathetic to his central thesis about the illusion of will, having previously advanced a similar proposal, Wegner's account of hypnosis is flawed. Hypnotic behavior derives from specific suggestions that are given, rather than from the induction, of trance, and it can be observed in 90% of the population. Thus, it is very pertinent to the illusion of will. However, Wegner exaggerates the loss of subjective will in hypnosis.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. Matter and spirit in the age of animal magnetism.Eric G. Wilson - 2006 - Philosophy and Literature 30 (2):329-345.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Matter and Spirit in the Age of Animal MagnetismEric G. WilsonDuring the Romantic period, writers on both sides of the Atlantic explored the sleepwalker as a merger of holiness and horror. Emerging when scientific thinkers for the first time were connecting spirit to electricity and magnetism, the somnambulist became to certain Romantics a disclosure of the difficulty of harmonizing unseen and seen, agency and necessity. This problem prominently arose (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  13.  16
    Facilitation of response to suggestion by response to previous suggestion of a different type.A. Jenness - 1933 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 16 (1):55.
  14.  17
    Hypnotic behavior dissected or … pulling the wings off butterflies.Dennis C. Turk & Thomas E. Rudy - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):485-485.
  15.  71
    Hypnotizing Libet: Readiness potentials with non-conscious volition.Alexander Schlegel, Prescott Alexander, Walter Sinnott-Armstrong, Adina Roskies, Peter Ulric Tse & Thalia Wheatley - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33 (C):196-203.
    The readiness potential (RP) is one of the most controversial topics in neuroscience and philosophy due to its perceived relevance to the role of conscious willing in action. Libet and colleagues reported that RP onset precedes both volitional movement and conscious awareness of willing that movement, suggesting that the experience of conscious will may not cause volitional movement (Libet, Gleason, Wright, & Pearl, 1983). Rather, they suggested that the RP indexes unconscious processes that may actually cause both volitional movement and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  16. Hypnotic suggestibility, cognitive inhibition, and dissociation.Zoltán Dienes, Elizabeth Brown, Sam Hutton, Irving Kirsch, Giuliana Mazzoni & Daniel B. Wright - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (4):837-847.
    We examined two potential correlates of hypnotic suggestibility: dissociation and cognitive inhibition. Dissociation is the foundation of two of the major theories of hypnosis and other theories commonly postulate that hypnotic responding is a result of attentional abilities . Participants were administered the Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C. Under the guise of an unrelated study, 180 of these participants also completed: a version of the Dissociative Experiences Scale that is normally distributed in non-clinical populations; (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  17.  40
    Hypnotic control of attention in the stroop task: A historical footnote.Colin M. MacLeod & Peter W. Sheehan - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (3):347-353.
    have recently provided a compelling demonstration of enhanced attentional control under post-hypnotic suggestion. Using the classic color-word interference paradigm, in which the task is to ignore a word and to name the color in which it is printed (e.g., RED in green, say ''green''), they gave a post-hypnotic instruction to participants that they would be unable to read. This eliminated Stroop interference in high suggestibility participants but did not alter interference in low suggestibility participants. replicated this pattern and (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  18.  40
    Hypnotic susceptibility, baseline attentional functioning, and the Stroop task.Sandro Rubichi, Federico Ricci, Roberto Padovani & Lorenzo Scaglietti - 2005 - Consciousness and Cognition 14 (2):296-303.
    According to the theoretical framework relating hypnosis to attention, baseline attentional functioning in highly hypnotizable individuals should be more efficient than in low hypnotizable individuals. However, previous studies did not find differences in Stroop-like tasks in which the measure indicative of the Stroop interference effect was based on response latencies. This study was designed to determine whether subjects with different levels of hypnotic susceptibility show differences in baseline attentional functioning. To assess this hypothesis, high, medium, and low hypnotizable subjects (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  19.  32
    Culture, Trance, and the Mind‐Brain.Richard J. Castillo - 1995 - Anthropology of Consciousness 6 (1):17-34.
  20.  53
    Hypnotic induction decreases anterior default mode activity.William J. McGeown, Giuliana Mazzoni, Annalena Venneri & Irving Kirsch - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (4):848-855.
    The ‘default mode’ network refers to cortical areas that are active in the absence of goal-directed activity. In previous studies, decreased activity in the ‘default mode’ has always been associated with increased activation in task-relevant areas. We show that the induction of hypnosis can reduce anterior default mode activity during rest without increasing activity in other cortical regions. We assessed brain activation patterns of high and low suggestible people while resting in the fMRI scanner and while engaged in visual tasks, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  21.  34
    Hypnotic ingroup–outgroup suggestion influences economic decision-making in an Ultimatum Game.Martin Brüne, Cumhur Tas, Julia Wischniewski, Anna Welpinghus, Christine Heinisch & Albert Newen - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (2):939-946.
    Studies in economic decision-making have demonstrated that individuals appreciate social values supporting equity and disapprove unfairness when distributing goods between two or more parties. However, this seems to critically depend on psychological mechanisms partly pertaining to the ingroup–outgroup distinction. Little is known as to what extent economic bargaining can be manipulated by means of psychological interventions such has hypnosis. Here we show that a hypnotic ingroup versus outgroup suggestion impacts the tolerance of unfairness in an Ultimatum Game. Specifically, the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  8
    Hypnotic State Modulates Sensorimotor Beta Rhythms During Real Movement and Motor Imagery.Sébastien Rimbert, Manuel Zaepffel, Pierre Riff, Perrine Adam & Laurent Bougrain - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:478341.
    The hypnosis technique is currently used in the medical field and influences directly the patient's state of relaxation, perception of the body and its visual imagination. There is evidence to suggest that hypnosis state may help patients to better achieve the task of motor imagination, which is central in rehabilitation protocols after a stroke. However, the hypnosis technique could also alter the activity in motor cortex. To the best of our knowledge, the impact of hypnosis on the EEG signal during (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  77
    Hypnotic suggestibility predicts the magnitude of the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect in a non-hypnotic context.Benjamin A. Parris & Zoltan Dienes - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (3):868-874.
    The present study investigated how the magnitude the word blindness suggestion effect on Stroop interference depended on hypnotic suggestibility when given as an imaginative suggestion and under conditions in which hypnosis was not mentioned. Hypnotic suggestibility is shown to be a significant predictor of the magnitude of the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect under these conditions. This is therefore the first study to show a linear relationship between the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect and hypnotic suggestibility across (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  24.  50
    Is hypnotic responding the strategic relinquishment of metacognition?Zoltán Dienes, Michael Beran, Johannes L. Brandl, Josef Perner & Joelle Proust - 2012 - In Michael Beran, Johannes Brandl, Josef Perner & Joëlle Proust (eds.), The Foundations of Metacognition. Oxford University Press.
  25. Hypnotic phenomena and altered states of consciousness: A multilevel framework of description and explanation.Sakari Kallio & Antti Revonsuo - 2003 - Contemporary Hypnosis 20 (3):111-164.
  26.  24
    Hypnotic induction is followed by state-like changes in the organization of EEG functional connectivity in the theta and beta frequency bands in high-hypnotically susceptible individuals.Graham A. Jamieson & Adrian P. Burgess - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  27.  20
    Trance, posture, and tobacco in the Casas Grandes shamanic tradition: Altered states of consciousness and the interaction effects of behavioral variables.Christine S. VanPool, Laura Lee, Paul Robear & Todd L. VanPool - 2024 - Anthropology of Consciousness 35 (1):75-95.
    Here, we describe how Casas Grandes Medio period (AD 1200 to 1450) shamanic practices of the North American Southwest used tobacco shamanism, a ritual stance called the Tennessee Diviner (TD) posture, and cultural expectations to generate trance experiences of soul flight and divination. We introduce a conceptual model that holds that specific trance experiences are the emergent result of human minds interacting with additional factors including entheogens, cultural expectations, physiological states, postures/movement, and sound/stimulation. Experimental and ethnographic evidence indicates (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  68
    Trance, Possession, Shamanism and Sex.I. M. Lewis - 2003 - Anthropology of Consciousness 14 (1):20-39.
    Altered States of Consciousness is an umbrella term applied in the study of psychological, sociological and religious phenomena that are regularly encountered experientially in the study of trance, possession, and shamanism, all of which have complex and problematic links with music. Beginning with trance, and stressing the pervasive sexual imagery invoked, this paper reviews the role ofASC in these three areas in the anthropology of religion.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  29.  13
    Hypnotic suggestion and the conditioned reflex.V. E. Fisher - 1932 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 15 (2):212.
  30. Hypnotic control of attention in the stroop task: A historical footnote.M. C. & W. P. - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (3):347-353.
    have recently provided a compelling demonstration of enhanced attentional control under post-hypnotic suggestion. Using the classic color-word interference paradigm, in which the task is to ignore a word and to name the color in which it is printed (e.g., RED in green, say ''green''), they gave a post-hypnotic instruction to participants that they would be unable to read. This eliminated Stroop interference in high suggestibility participants but did not alter interference in low suggestibility participants. replicated this pattern and (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  6
    Can hypnotic susceptibility be explained by bifactor models? Structural equation modeling of the Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility – Form A.Anoushiravan Zahedi & Werner Sommer - 2022 - Consciousness and Cognition 99 (C):103289.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  23
    Trance States: A Theoretical Model and Cross‐Cultural Analysis.Michael Winkelman - 1986 - Ethos: Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology 14 (2):174-203.
  33.  14
    Hypnotic behaviour revisited: A trait-context interaction.Joseph Glicksohn - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):774.
  34.  16
    Hypnotic involuntariness: A social cognitive analysis.Steven J. Lynn, Judith W. Rhue & John R. Weekes - 1990 - Psychological Review 97 (2):169-184.
  35.  14
    Hypnotic Inductions: On the Persistence of the Subject: A Response to James Marshall.Bernadette Baker - 2007 - Foucault Studies 4:127-148.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36. Hypnotic regulation of consciousness and the pain neuromatrix.Melanie Boly, Marie-Elisabeth Faymonville, Brent A. Vogt, Pierre Maquet & Laureys & Steven - 2007 - In Graham Jamieson (ed.), Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Oxford University Press.
  37.  19
    Trance types and amnesia revisited: Using detailed interviews to fill in the gaps.Sarasvati Buhrman - 1997 - Anthropology of Consciousness 8 (1):10-21.
  38.  31
    Hypnosis, hypnotic suggestibility, memory, and involvement in films.Reed Maxwell, Steven Jay Lynn & Liam Condon - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33:170-184.
  39.  60
    Is hypnotic suggestibility a stable trait?☆.Oliver Fassler, Steven Jay Lynn & Joshua Knox - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):240-253.
    The present study examined the trait-like nature of hypnotic suggestibility by examining the stability of hypnotic responsiveness in a test–retest design in which the procedures were administered either live or by audiotape. Contrary to the idea that hypnotizability is a largely immutable, stable trait, scores on the scale of hypnotic responsiveness decreased significantly at the second session. Measures of subjective experiences and expectancies accounted for a sizable portion of the variance in hypnotic responding, both at initial (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  40.  25
    Furnishing hypnotic instructions with implementation intentions enhances hypnotic responsiveness.Inge Schweiger Gallo, Florian Pfau & Peter M. Gollwitzer - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (2):1023-1030.
    Forming implementation intentions has been consistently shown to be a powerful self-regulatory strategy. As the self-regulation of thoughts is important for the experience of involuntariness in the hypnotic context, investigating the effectiveness of implementation intentions on the suppression of thoughts was the focus of the present study. Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions . Results showed that participants who received information included in the “Carleton Skill Training Program” and in addition formed implementation intentions improved their (...) responsiveness as compared to all of the other three groups on measures of objective responding and involuntary responding. Thus, in line with the nonstate or cognitive social–psychological view of hypnosis stating that an individual’s hypnotic suggestibility is not dispositional but modifiable, our results suggest that hypnotic responsiveness can be heightened by furnishing hypnotic instructions with ad hoc implementation intentions. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  48
    Hypnotic responding and self-deception.Irving Kirsch - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (1):118-119.
    As understood by neodissociation and sociocognitive theorists, hypnotic responses are instances of self-deception. Neodissociation theory matches the strict definition of Sackeim and Gur (1978) and sociocognitive theory matches Mele's looser definition. Recent data indicate that many hypnotized individuals deceive themselves into holding conflicting beliefs without dissociating, but others convince themselves that the suggested state of affairs is true without simultaneously holding a contrary belief.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  21
    The hypnotic induction of hallucinatory color vision followed by pseudo-negative after-images.Milton H. Erickson & Elizabeth Moore Erickson - 1938 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 22 (6):581.
  43.  9
    Hypnotic and non-hypnotic suggestion to ignore pre-cues decreases space-valence congruency effects in highly hypnotizable individuals.Ya Zhang, Yan Wang & Yixuan Ku - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 65:293-303.
  44.  22
    Trance, Dissociation, and Shamanism: A Cross-Cultural Model.Connor Wood, Saikou Diallo, Ross Gore & Christopher J. Lynch - 2018 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 18 (5):508-536.
    Religious practices centered on controlled trance states, such as Siberian shamanism or North African zar, are ubiquitous, yet their characteristics vary. In particular, cross-cultural research finds that female-dominated spirit possession cults are common in stratified societies, whereas male-dominated shamanism predominates in structurally flatter cultures. Here, we present an agent-based model that explores factors, including social stratification and psychological dissociation, that may partially account for this pattern. We posit that, in more stratified societies, female agents suffer from higher levels of (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. Trance.Imants Baruss - 2003 - In Mind. American Psychological Association. pp. 135-159.
  46. Trance.Beard Beard - 1877 - Mind 2:568.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  17
    Hypnotic state: An interminable controversy.Léon Chertok - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):773.
  48. Sedative-, hypnotic-, or anxiolytic-related disorders. Abuse liability.A. C. Domenic & S. Ofra - forthcoming - Human Studies. In: Kaplan and Sadock’s Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry.
  49.  12
    Hypnotic susceptibility, EEG-alpha, and self-regulation.David R. Engstrom - 1976 - In Gary E. Schwartz & D. H. Shapiro (eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation. Plenum. pp. 173--221.
  50.  14
    Hypnotically induced mood.Rena Friswell & Kevin M. McConkey - 1989 - Cognition and Emotion 3 (1):1-26.
1 — 50 / 328