Results for ' motor image6'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Christian Mannes.Learning Sensory-Motor Coordination Experimentation - 1990 - In G. Dorffner (ed.), Konnektionismus in Artificial Intelligence Und Kognitionsforschung. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. pp. 95.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2. Stimuli and instructions.Visaud Somat, Vis Vis, J. L_ & Motor Plants - 1986 - In David A. Oakley (ed.), Mind and Brain. Methuen.
  3.  8
    Values Evolution in Human Machine Relations: Grounding Computationalism and Neural Dynamics in a Physical a Priorism of Nature.Denis Larrivee - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15:649544.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4. Motor imagery and action execution.Bence Nanay - 2020 - Ergo: An Open Access Journal of Philosophy.
    What triggers the execution of actions? What happens in that moment when an action is triggered? What mental state is there at the moment of action-execution that was not there a second before? My aim is to highlight the importance of a thus far largely ignored kind of mental state in the discussion of these old and much-debated questions: motor imagery. While there have been a fair amount of research in psychology and neuroscience on motor imagery in the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  5.  53
    Motor cortex fields and speech movements: Simple dual control is implausible.James H. Abbs & Roxanne DePaul - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (4):511-512.
    We applaud the spirit of MacNeilage's attempts to better explain the evolution and cortical control of speech by drawing on the vast literature in nonhuman primate neurobiology. However, he oversimplifies motor cortical fields and their known individual functions to such an extent that he undermines the value of his effort. In particular, MacNeilage has lumped together the functional characteristics across multiple mesial and lateral motor cortex fields, inadvertantly creating two hypothetical centers that simply may not exist.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  7
    Il motore della mente: il movimento nella storia delle scienze cognitive.Carmela Morabito - 2020 - Bari: GLF editori Laterza.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7. Motor Intentions: How Intentions and Motor Representations Come Together.Chiara Brozzo - 2017 - Mind and Language 32 (2):231-256.
    What are the most detailed descriptions under which subjects intend to perform bodily actions? According to Pacherie (2006), these descriptions may be found by looking into motor representations—action representations in the brain that determine the movements to be performed. Specifically, for any motor representation guiding an action, its subject has an M‐intention representing that action in as much detail. I show that some M‐intentions breach the constraints that intentions should meet. I then identify a set of intentions—motor (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  8.  90
    Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell the Self.Marc Jeannerod - 2006 - Oxford University Press.
    Our ability to acknowledge and recognise our own identity - our 'self' - is a characteristic doubtless unique to humans. Where does this feeling come from? How does the combination of neurophysiological processes coupled with our interaction with the outside world construct this coherent identity? We know that our social interactions contribute via the eyes, ears etc. However, our self is not only influenced by our senses. It is also influenced by the actions we perform and those we see others (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   170 citations  
  9. Motor Imagery and Merleau-Pontyian Accounts of Skilled Action.J. C. Berendzen - 2014 - Ergo: An Open Access Journal of Philosophy 1:169-198.
    Maurice Merleau-Ponty is often interpreted as claiming that opportunities for action are directly present in perceptual experience. However, he does not provide much evidence for how or why this would occur, and one can doubt that this is an appropriate interpretation of his phenomenological descriptions. In particular, it could be argued the Merleau-Pontyian descriptions mistakenly attribute pre-perceptual or post-perceptual elements such as allocation of attention or judgment to the perceptual experience itself. This paper argues for the Merleau-Pontyian idea that opportunities (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  10. Motor intentionality and the case of Schneider.Rasmus Thybo Jensen - 2009 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 8 (3):371-388.
    I argue that Merleau-Ponty’s use of the case of Schneider in his arguments for the existence of non-conconceptual and non-representational motor intentionality contains a problematic methodological ambiguity. Motor intentionality is both to be revealed by its perspicuous preservation and by its contrastive impairment in one and the same case. To resolve the resulting contradiction I suggest we emphasize the second of Merleau-Ponty’s two lines of argument. I argue that this interpretation is the one in best accordance both with (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  11. Ideo-motor actions: An embodied account.Matteo Baccarini - 2011 - Annales Philosophici 3:22-29.
    The ideo-motor theory of actions, which has been strongly criticized during the last century, is currently receiving new interest in both cognitive psychology and philosophy. In this paper I will describe the principal theoretical reasons of this renewed attention. More particularly, here I will propose that the functioning of the mirror neurons’ system is a very useful tool to describe how the classic concept of ideo-motor action is close to the contemporary concept of simulated action.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  21
    Motor Action and Emotional Memory.Daniel Casasanto & Katinka Dijkstra - 2010 - Cognition 115 (1):179.
  13.  80
    Supplementary motor area structure and function: review and hypotheses.Gary Goldberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):567-588.
  14. Motor experience interacts with effector information during action prediction.Lincoln Colling, William Thompson & John Sutton - 2013 - Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society:2082-2087.
    Recent theory suggests that action prediction relies of a motor emulation mechanism that works by mapping observed actions onto the observer action system so that predictions can be generated using that same predictive mechanisms that underlie action control. This suggests that action prediction may be more accurate when there is a more direct mapping between the stimulus and the observer. We tested this hypothesis by comparing prediction accuracy for two stimulus types. A mannequin stimulus which contained information about the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15.  32
    Motor processes in mental rotation.Mark Wexler, Stephen M. Kosslyn & Alain Berthoz - 1998 - Cognition 68 (1):77-94.
    Much indirect evidence supports the hypothesis that transformations of mental images are at least in part guided by motor processes, even in the case of images of abstract objects rather than of body parts. For example, rotation may be guided by processes that also prime one to see results of a specific motor action. We directly test the hypothesis by means of a dual-task paradigm in which subjects perform the Cooper-Shepard mental rotation task while executing an unseen (...) rotation in a given direction and at a previously learned speed. Four results support the inference that mental rotation relies on motor processes. First, motor rotation that is compatible with mental rotation results in faster times and fewer errors in the imagery task than when the two rotations are incompatible. Second, the angle through which subjects rotate their mental images, and the angle through which they rotate a joystick handle are correlated, but only if the directions of the two rotations are compatible. Third, motor rotation modifies the classical inverted V-shaped mental rotation response time function, favoring the direction of the motor rotation; indeed, in some cases motor rotation even shifts the location of the minimum of this curve in the direction of the motor rotation. Fourth, the preceding effect is sensitive not only to the direction of the motor rotation, but also to the motor speed. A change in the speed of motor rotation can correspondingly slow down or speed up the mental rotation. (shrink)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   60 citations  
  16.  41
    Motor intentionality and the intentionality of improvisation: a contribution to a phenomenology of musical improvisation.Lucia Angelino - 2018 - Continental Philosophy Review 52 (2):203-224.
    The intentionality of improvisation represents surely one of the most pressing and controversial issues in contemporary action theory: how do we find the way to characterize the proper intentionality of improvisation, which is an unplanned yet intentional action? This article will address this question bringing together Merleau-Ponty’s motor intentionality and Bergson’s conception of duration. My argument will unfold in three main stages. First, I will briefly describe the traditional scheme that is used to think of intentional action in contemporary (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  17. The Motor Theory of Speech Perception.Christopher Mole - 2009 - In Matthew Nudds & Casey O'Callaghan (eds.), Sounds and Perception: New Philosophical Essays. Oxford University Press.
    There is a long‐standing project in psychology the goal of which is to explain our ability to perceive speech. The project is motivated by evidence that seems to indicate that the cognitive processing to which speech sounds are subjected is somehow different from the normal processing employed in hearing. The Motor Theory of speech perception was proposed in the 1960s as an attempt to explain this specialness. The first part of this essay is concerned with the Motor Theory's (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  18.  43
    Motor Area Activity During Mental Rotation Studied by Time-Resolved Single-Trial fMRI.Wolfgang Richter, Randy Summers, Seong-Gi Kim & Carola Tegeler - unknown
    & The functional equivalence of overt movements and dynamic imagery is of fundamental importance in neuroscience. Here, we investigated the participation of the neocortical motor areas in a classic task of dynamic imagery, Shepard and Metzler's mental rotation task, by time-resolved single-trial functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI). The subjects performed the mental-rotation task 16 times, each time with different object pairs. Functional images were acquired for each pair separately, and the onset times and..
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  19. Thoughts, motor actions, and the self.Gottfried Vosgerau & Albert Newen - 2007 - Mind and Language 22 (1):22–43.
    The comparator-model, originally developed to explain motor action, has recently been invoked to explain several aspects of the self. However, in the first place it may not be used to explain a basic self-world distinction because it presupposes one. Our alternative account is based on specific systematic covariation between action and perception. Secondly, the comparator model cannot explain the feeling of ownership of thoughts. We argue—contra Frith and Campbell—that thoughts are not motor processes and therefore cannot be described (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  20.  50
    Motor awareness without perceptual awareness.Helen Johnson & Patrick Haggard - 2005 - Neuropsychologia. Special Issue 43 (2):227-237.
    The control of action has traditionally been described as "automatic". In particular, movement control may occur without conscious awareness, in contrast to normal visual perception. Studies on rapid visuomotor adjustment of reaching movements following a target shift have played a large part in introducing such distinctions. We suggest that previous studies of the relation between motor performance and perceptual awareness have confounded two separate dissociations. These are: (a) the distinction between motoric and perceptual representations, and (b) an orthogonal distinction (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  21. The motor theory of social cognition: a critique.Pierre Jacob & Marc Jeannerod - 2005 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (1):21-25.
    Recent advances in the cognitive neuroscience of action have considerably enlarged our understanding of human motor cognition. In particular, the activity of the mirror system, first discovered in the brain of non-human primates, provides an observer with the understanding of a perceived action by means of the motor simulation of the agent's observed movements. This discovery has raised the prospects of a motor theory of social cognition. Since human social cognition includes the ability to mindread, many (...) theorists of social cognition try to bridge the gap between motor cognition and mindreading by endorsing a simulation account of mindreading. Here, we express our skepticism about the motor theory of social cognition. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   79 citations  
  22.  94
    Plasticity, motor intentionality and concrete movement in Merleau-Ponty.Timothy Mooney - 2011 - Continental Philosophy Review 44 (4):359-381.
    Merleau-Ponty’s explication of concrete or practical movement by way of the Schneider case could be read as ending up close to automatism, neglecting its flexibility and plasticity in the face of obstacles. It can be contended that he already goes off course in his explication of Schneider’s condition. Rasmus Jensen has argued that he assimilates a normal person’s motor intentionality to the patient’s, thereby generating a vacuity problem. I argue that Schneider’s difficulties with certain movements point to a means (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  23. Intention and Motor Representation in Purposive Action.Stephen Andrew Butterfill & Corrado Sinigaglia - 2012 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 88 (1):119-145.
    Are there distinct roles for intention and motor representation in explaining the purposiveness of action? Standard accounts of action assign a role to intention but are silent on motor representation. The temptation is to suppose that nothing need be said here because motor representation is either only an enabling condition for purposive action or else merely a variety of intention. This paper provides reasons for resisting that temptation. Some motor representations, like intentions, coordinate actions in virtue (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   133 citations  
  24. Motor Intentions and Non‐Observational Knowledge of Action: A Standard Story.Olle Blomberg & Chiara Brozzo - 2017 - Thought: A Journal of Philosophy 6 (3):137-146.
    According to the standard story given by reductive versions of the Causal Theory of Action, an action is an intrinsically mindless bodily movement that is appropriately caused by an intention. Those who embrace this story typically take this intention to have a coarse-grained content, specifying the action only down to the level of the agent's habits and skills. Markos Valaris argues that, because of this, the standard story cannot make sense of the deep reach of our non-observational knowledge of action. (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  25.  17
    Motor effects of strong auditory stimuli.Roland C. Davis - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (3):257.
  26. Motor Control: Models.Liana E. Brown & David A. Rosenbaum - 2002 - In Lynn Nadel (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Macmillan.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  27.  24
    Motor intentionality and the intentionality of improvisation: a contribution to a phenomenology of musical improvisation.Lucia Angelino - 2018 - Continental Philosophy Review 52 (2):203-224.
    The intentionality of improvisation represents surely one of the most pressing and controversial issues in contemporary action theory: how do we find the way to characterize the proper intentionality of improvisation, which is an unplanned yet intentional action? This article will address this question bringing together Merleau-Ponty’s motor intentionality and Bergson’s conception of duration. My argument will unfold in three main stages. First, I will briefly describe the traditional scheme that is used to think of intentional action in contemporary (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  28.  34
    Motor representation in acting together.Corrado Sinigaglia & Stephen A. Butterfill - 2022 - Synthese 200 (2):1-16.
    People walk, build, paint and otherwise act together with a purpose in myriad ways. What is the relation between the actions people perform in acting together with a purpose and the outcome, or outcomes, to which their actions are directed? We argue that fully characterising this relation will require appeal not only to intention, knowledge and other familiar philosophical paraphernalia but also to another kind of representation involved in preparing and executing actions, namely motor representation. If we are right, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  29. Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts.Jason Stanley & John W. Krakauer - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  30.  19
    Motor-sensory feedback and geometry of visual space: an attempted replication.John Gyr, Richmond Willey & Adele Henry - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):59-64.
  31.  27
    Drawn together: When motor representations ground joint actions.Francesco Della Gatta, Francesca Garbarini, Marco Rabuffetti, Luca Viganò, Stephen A. Butterfill & Corrado Sinigaglia - 2017 - Cognition 165 (C):53-60.
    What enables individuals to act together? Recent discoveries suggest that a variety of mechanisms are involved. But something fundamental is yet to be investigated. In joint action, agents represent a collective goal, or so it is often assumed. But how, if at all, are collective goals represented in joint action and how do such representations impact performance? To investigate this question we adapted a bimanual paradigm, the circle-line drawing paradigm, to contrast two agents acting in parallel with two agents performing (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  32.  14
    Cognitive, Motor and Social Factors of Music Instrument Training Programs for Older Adults’ Improved Wellbeing.Jennifer MacRitchie, Matthew Breaden, Andrew J. Milne & Sarah McIntyre - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Given emerging evidence that learning to play a musical instrument may lead to a number of cognitive benefits for older adults, it is important to clarify how these training programs can be delivered optimally and meaningfully. The effective acquisition of musical and domain-general skills by later-life learners may be influenced by social, cultural and individual factors within the learning environment. The current study examines the effects of a 10-week piano training program on healthy older adult novices’ cognitive and motor (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  33.  14
    Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts.Richard A. Schmidt - 1979 - Psychological Review 86 (5):415-451.
  34.  14
    Motor control of serial ordering of speech.Peter F. MacNeilage - 1970 - Psychological Review 77 (3):182-196.
  35.  5
    Motor Simulation and Ostensive-Inferential Communication.Angelo D. Delliponti - 2022 - Avant: Trends in Interdisciplinary Studies 13 (1).
    The ostensive-inferential model is a model of communication, an alternative to the code model of communication, based on pragmatic competence: it explains human communication in terms of expression and recognition of informative and communicative intentions, founding comprehension on the distinction between literal meaning and the speaker’s meaning. Through informative intentions we try to make evident the content of a message to a receiver, or to make evident what we want to communicate to him/her: communicative intentions are used to make evident (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  10
    Motor Imagery and Action Observation as Appropriate Strategies for Home-Based Rehabilitation: A Mini-Review Focusing on Improving Physical Function in Orthopedic Patients.Armin H. Paravlic - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Dynamic stability of the knee and weakness of the extensor muscles are considered to be the most important functional limitations after anterior cruciate ligament injury, probably due to changes at the central level of motor control rather than at the peripheral level. Despite general technological advances, fewer contraindicative surgical procedures, and extensive postoperative rehabilitation, up to 65% of patients fail to return to their preinjury level of sports, and only half were able to return to competitive sport. Later, it (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  37. Motor ontology: The representational reality of goals, actions and selves.Vittorio Gallese & Thomas Metzinger - 2003 - Philosophical Psychology 16 (3):365 – 388.
    The representational dynamics of the brain is a subsymbolic process, and it has to be conceived as an "agent-free" type of dynamical self-organization. However, in generating a coherent internal world-model, the brain decomposes target space in a certain way. In doing so, it defines an "ontology": to have an ontology is to interpret a world. In this paper we argue that the brain, viewed as a representational system aimed at interpreting the world, possesses an ontology too. It decomposes target space (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  38. Categorically Perceiving Motor Actions.Chiara Brozzo - 2020 - In Daniel Weiskopf (ed.), Neural Mechanisms: New Challenges in the Philosophy of Neuroscience. pp. 465-482.
    In this chapter, I will present an empirical conjecture to the effect that some bodily actions are categorically perceived. These are bodily actions such as grasping or reaching for something, which I am going to call motor actions. My conjecture builds on one recently put forward about how the categorical perception of facial expressions of some emotions works. I shall motivate my own conjecture on the basis of both theoretical and empirical considerations, describe how it could be operationalised and (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  15
    Modulating Motor Learning through Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation: An Integrative View.Claudia Ammann, Danny Spampinato & Javier Márquez-Ruiz - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  40.  62
    The motor theory of speech perception revised.Alvin M. Liberman & Ignatius G. Mattingly - 1985 - Cognition 21 (1):1-36.
  41.  22
    Motor conflict behavior as a function of motivation and amount of training.Burton G. Andreas - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (2):173.
  42.  66
    Motor Skill and Moral Virtue.Ellen Fridland - 2017 - Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 80:139-170.
    Virtue ethicists often appeal to practical skill as a way of understanding the nature of virtue. An important commitment of a skill account of virtue is that virtue is learned through practice and not through study, memorization, or reflection alone. In what follows, I will argue that virtue ethicists have only given us half the story. In particular, in focusing on outputs, or on the right actions or responses to moral situations, virtue ethicists have overlooked a crucial facet of virtue: (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  43.  14
    Motor and Predictive Processes in Auditory Beat and Rhythm Perception.Shannon Proksch, Daniel C. Comstock, Butovens Médé, Alexandria Pabst & Ramesh Balasubramaniam - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  44. Skill and motor control: intelligence all the way down.Ellen Fridland - 2017 - Philosophical Studies 174 (6):1-22.
    When reflecting on the nature of skilled action, it is easy to fall into familiar dichotomies such that one construes the flexibility and intelligence of skill at the level of intentional states while characterizing the automatic motor processes that constitute motor skill execution as learned but fixed, invariant, bottom-up, brute-causal responses. In this essay, I will argue that this picture of skilled, automatic, motor processes is overly simplistic. Specifically, I will argue that an adequate account of the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  45.  4
    Motor Processes and Mental Unity.J. Mark Baldwin - 1909 - Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods 6 (7):182-185.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  15
    Motor-Kurye Çalışanlarının Çalışma Koşullarına Yönelik Bir Araştırma: İstanbul Örneği.Umut Yertüm & Bayram Balci - 2023 - Akademik İncelemeler Dergisi 18 (2):328-350.
    Bu çalışma İstanbul’da faaliyette bulunan motor-kuryelerin çalışma koşulları, ücret düzeyleri, yasal hakları, iş kazası ve meslek hastalıkları ve karşılaşılan ayrımcılıkların araştırılması amaçlanmıştır. Bu amaçla, 20 motor-kurye ile yarı yapılandırılmış mülakat tekniğiyle görüşmeler gerçekleştirilmiştir. Yapılan görüşmeler sonucunda, motor-kuryelik mesleğinde esnaf ve bordrolu olmak üzere ikili bir yapı oluştuğu; katılımcıların %68’i esnaf, %37’si ise bordrolu olarak çalıştığı görülmektedir. Bordrolu çalışan motor-kuryelerin ücretli izin hakkı bulunurken; esnaf kuryelerin tamamının ücretli izin haklarının olmadığı, %61’ inin ise sosyal güvenceye sahip olmadığı (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. Naturalizing motor control theory: Isn't it time for a new paradigm?Garrett E. Alexander, Mahlon R. DeLong & Michael D. Crutcher - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):828-833.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  26
    Vicarious motor activation during action perception: beyond correlational evidence.Alessio Avenanti, Matteo Candidi & Cosimo Urgesi - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  49. The modularity of the motor system.Myrto Mylopoulos - 2021 - Philosophical Explorations 24 (3):376-393.
    In this paper, I make a case for the modularity of the motor system. I start where many do in discussions of modularity, by considering the extent to which the motor system is cognitively penetrable, i.e., the extent to which its processing and outputs are causally influenced, in a semantically coherent way, by states of central cognition. I present some empirical findings from a range of sensorimotor adaptation studies that strongly suggest that there are limits to such influence (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  50.  21
    Complex Motor Learning and Police Training: Applied, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives.Paula M. Di Nota & Juha-Matti Huhta - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000