Results for ' motor'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Christian Mannes.Learning Sensory-Motor Coordination Experimentation - 1990 - In G. Dorffner (ed.), Konnektionismus in Artificial Intelligence Und Kognitionsforschung. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. pp. 95.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  34
    Catatonia, motor neglect, and hysterical paralysis: Some similarities and differences.John C. Marshall, Jennifer M. Gurd & Gereon R. Fink - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (5):587-588.
    We outline some ways in which motor neglect (the underutilization of a limb despite adequate strength) and hysterical paralysis (failure to move a limb despite no relevant structural damage or disease) may throw light on the pathophysiology of catatonia. We also comment on the manifold inadequacies of distinguishing too firmly between symptoms of “neurologic origin” and of “psychiatric origin.”.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3. Stimuli and instructions.Visaud Somat, Vis Vis, J. L_ & Motor Plants - 1986 - In David A. Oakley (ed.), Mind and Brain. Methuen.
  4. Do cortical and basal ganglionic motor areas use “motor programs” to control movement?Garrett E. Alexander, Mahlon R. DeLong & Michael D. Crutcher - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):656-665.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   75 citations  
  5. The Relationship between Motor Skills, Perceived Social Support, and Internalizing Problems in a Community Adolescent Sample.Vincent O. Mancini, Daniela Rigoli, Brody Heritage, Lynne D. Roberts & Jan P. Piek - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  9
    Approximate Optimal Control as a Model for Motor Learning.Neil E. Berthier, Michael T. Rosenstein & Andrew G. Barto - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (2):329-346.
  7. Association Between Preschoolers’ Specific Fine (But Not Gross) Motor Skills and Later Academic Competencies: Educational Implications.Elena Escolano-Pérez, Maria Luisa Herrero-Nivela & José Luis Losada - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  8.  41
    Role of kinesthetic and spatial-visual abilities in perceptual-motor learning.Edwin A. Fleishman & Simon Rich - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (1):6.
  9. Motor imagery and action execution.Bence Nanay - 2020 - Ergo: An Open Access Journal of Philosophy.
    What triggers the execution of actions? What happens in that moment when an action is triggered? What mental state is there at the moment of action-execution that was not there a second before? My aim is to highlight the importance of a thus far largely ignored kind of mental state in the discussion of these old and much-debated questions: motor imagery. While there have been a fair amount of research in psychology and neuroscience on motor imagery in the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  10. EMDR therapy for PTSD after motor vehicle accidents: meta-analytic evidence for specific treatment.Maddalena Boccia, Laura Piccardi, Pierluigi Cordellieri, Cecilia Guariglia & Anna Maria Giannini - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  11.  33
    Factors in the retention and relearning of perceptual-motor skill.Edwin A. Fleishman & James F. Parker - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (3):215.
  12. Motor Intentions: How Intentions and Motor Representations Come Together.Chiara Brozzo - 2017 - Mind and Language 32 (2):231-256.
    What are the most detailed descriptions under which subjects intend to perform bodily actions? According to Pacherie (2006), these descriptions may be found by looking into motor representations—action representations in the brain that determine the movements to be performed. Specifically, for any motor representation guiding an action, its subject has an M‐intention representing that action in as much detail. I show that some M‐intentions breach the constraints that intentions should meet. I then identify a set of intentions—motor (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  13. Gas fuelled engines like ecological alternative for driving of motor vehicles.Stanislav Beroun, Ladislav BARTONÍČEK, Josef Laurin & Celestýn Scholz - 2005 - In Alan F. Blackwell & David MacKay (eds.), Power. Cambridge University Press. pp. 1.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  24
    A "partial reinforcement extinction effect" in perceptual-motor performance: Coerced versus volunteer subject populations.Roger W. Black, Joseph Schumpert & Frances Welch - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (1):143.
  15. The role of visual experience in senso-motor integration.I. Blinnikova - 2000 - Consciousness and Cognition 9 (2):S98 - S98.
  16.  6
    Más allá de la seguridad y la libertad. La solidaridad como motor alternativo de las políticas migratorias.Diego Blázquez Martín - 2005 - Arbor 181 (713):143-153.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  9
    Cats Parallel Great Apes and Corvids in Motor Self-Regulation – Not Brain but Material Size Matters.Katarzyna Bobrowicz & Mathias Osvath - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  18
    Use of negative slope transformations of knowledge of results on a simple motor response.Donald A. Schumsky - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (5):534.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  22
    Bilateral integrative action of the cerebral cortex in man in verbal association and sensori-motor coordination.Karl U. Smith - 1947 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 37 (5):367.
  20.  49
    Prior positioning responses as a factor in short-term retention of a simple motor task.George E. Stelmach - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (3):523.
  21. Motor intentionality and the case of Schneider.Rasmus Thybo Jensen - 2009 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 8 (3):371-388.
    I argue that Merleau-Ponty’s use of the case of Schneider in his arguments for the existence of non-conconceptual and non-representational motor intentionality contains a problematic methodological ambiguity. Motor intentionality is both to be revealed by its perspicuous preservation and by its contrastive impairment in one and the same case. To resolve the resulting contradiction I suggest we emphasize the second of Merleau-Ponty’s two lines of argument. I argue that this interpretation is the one in best accordance both with (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  22.  15
    Partial visual feedback of component motions as a function of difficulty of motor control.John D. Gould & Amy Schaffer - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (6):564.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  15
    Investigating the Role of the Primary Motor Cortex in Musical Creativity: A Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Study.Aydin Anic, Kirk N. Olsen & William Forde Thompson - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  8
    Conscious awareness of others’ actions during observational learning does not benefit motor skill performance.Arnaud Badets, Camille Jeunet, Françoise Dellu-Hagedorn, Mélissa Ployart, Sandra Chanraud & Arnaud Boutin - 2023 - Consciousness and Cognition 113 (C):103553.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  18
    The effects of pacing and distribution on intercorrelations of motor abilities.R. D. Nance - 1947 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 37 (6):459.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  34
    Acquisition and long-term retention of a simple serial perceptual-motor skill.Eva Neumann & R. B. Ammons - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (3):159.
  27.  28
    Sense of body ownership in patients affected by functional motor symptoms.Benedetta Demartini, Lucia Ricciardi, Laura Crucianelli, Aikaterini Fotopoulou & Mark J. Edwards - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 39:70-76.
  28. The prp effect-central bottleneck, perceptual-motor limitations, or task strategies.De de MeyerKieras - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (6):475-475.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  18
    Performance decrement in a simple motor task before and after a single rest.Edward A. Bilodeau - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 43 (5):381.
  30.  21
    Rate recovery in a repetitive motor task as a function of successive rest periods.Edward A. Bilodeau - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (3):197.
  31.  24
    Some effects of work loading in a repetitive motor task.Ina McD Bilodeau & Edward A. Bilodeau - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (6):455.
  32.  30
    Effect of addition of irrelevant verbal cues on perceptual-motor learning.Harry W. Braun & A. W. Bendig - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 54 (2):105.
  33.  37
    Comparison of the effect of auditory versus visual stimulation on information capacity of discrete motor responses.W. W. Breen, M. J. De Haemer & G. K. Poock - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (2):395.
  34.  18
    A multiple groove board for testing motor skill.S. K. Chou - 1929 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 12 (3):249.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  17
    A two-process theory for the short-term retention of motor responses.John L. Craft - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (1):196.
  36.  18
    Identification and discrimination functions for a visual continuum and their relation to the motor theory of speech perception.D. V. Cross, H. L. Lane & W. C. Sheppard - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (1):63.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  19
    An electro-kymograph used in recording vaso-motor changes.S. B. Lindley - 1928 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 11 (4):325.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38. The Motor Theory of Speech Perception.Christopher Mole - 2009 - In Matthew Nudds & Casey O'Callaghan (eds.), Sounds and Perception: New Philosophical Essays. Oxford University Press.
    There is a long‐standing project in psychology the goal of which is to explain our ability to perceive speech. The project is motivated by evidence that seems to indicate that the cognitive processing to which speech sounds are subjected is somehow different from the normal processing employed in hearing. The Motor Theory of speech perception was proposed in the 1960s as an attempt to explain this specialness. The first part of this essay is concerned with the Motor Theory's (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  39.  72
    Value-based modulation of effort and reward anticipation on the motor system.Vassena Eliana, Cobbaert Stephanie, Andres Michael, Fias Wim & Verguts Tom - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  40. Tool use and the concept of sensori-motor stages.As Etienne - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):594-594.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  90
    Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell the Self.Marc Jeannerod - 2006 - Oxford University Press.
    Our ability to acknowledge and recognise our own identity - our 'self' - is a characteristic doubtless unique to humans. Where does this feeling come from? How does the combination of neurophysiological processes coupled with our interaction with the outside world construct this coherent identity? We know that our social interactions contribute via the eyes, ears etc. However, our self is not only influenced by our senses. It is also influenced by the actions we perform and those we see others (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   170 citations  
  42. Intentions and Motor Representations: the Interface Challenge.Myrto Mylopoulos & Elisabeth Pacherie - 2017 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 8 (2):317-336.
    A full account of purposive action must appeal not only to propositional attitude states like beliefs, desires, and intentions, but also to motor representations, i.e., non-propositional states that are thought to represent, among other things, action outcomes as well as detailed kinematic features of bodily movements. This raises the puzzle of how it is that these two distinct types of state successfully coordinate. We examine this so-called “Interface Problem”. First, we clarify and expand on the nature and role of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   71 citations  
  43.  22
    Auditory-motor synchronization with temporally fluctuating sequences is dependent on fractal structure but not musical expertise.Summer K. Rankin & Charles J. Limb - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:103164.
    Fractal structure is a ubiquitous property found in nature and biology, and has been observed in processes at different levels of organization, including rhythmic behavior and musical structure. A temporal process is characterized as fractal when serial long-term correlations and statistical self-similarity (scaling) are present. Previous studies of sensorimotor synchronization using isochronous (non-fractal) stimuli show that participants' errors exhibit persistent structure (positive long-term correlations), while their inter-tap intervals (ITIs) exhibit anti-persistent structure (negative long-term correlations). Auditory-motor synchronization has not been (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  50
    Motor awareness without perceptual awareness.Helen Johnson & Patrick Haggard - 2005 - Neuropsychologia. Special Issue 43 (2):227-237.
    The control of action has traditionally been described as "automatic". In particular, movement control may occur without conscious awareness, in contrast to normal visual perception. Studies on rapid visuomotor adjustment of reaching movements following a target shift have played a large part in introducing such distinctions. We suggest that previous studies of the relation between motor performance and perceptual awareness have confounded two separate dissociations. These are: (a) the distinction between motoric and perceptual representations, and (b) an orthogonal distinction (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  45. The motor theory of social cognition: a critique.Pierre Jacob & Marc Jeannerod - 2005 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (1):21-25.
    Recent advances in the cognitive neuroscience of action have considerably enlarged our understanding of human motor cognition. In particular, the activity of the mirror system, first discovered in the brain of non-human primates, provides an observer with the understanding of a perceived action by means of the motor simulation of the agent's observed movements. This discovery has raised the prospects of a motor theory of social cognition. Since human social cognition includes the ability to mindread, many (...) theorists of social cognition try to bridge the gap between motor cognition and mindreading by endorsing a simulation account of mindreading. Here, we express our skepticism about the motor theory of social cognition. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   79 citations  
  46. Motor Imagery and Merleau-Pontyian Accounts of Skilled Action.J. C. Berendzen - 2014 - Ergo: An Open Access Journal of Philosophy 1:169-198.
    Maurice Merleau-Ponty is often interpreted as claiming that opportunities for action are directly present in perceptual experience. However, he does not provide much evidence for how or why this would occur, and one can doubt that this is an appropriate interpretation of his phenomenological descriptions. In particular, it could be argued the Merleau-Pontyian descriptions mistakenly attribute pre-perceptual or post-perceptual elements such as allocation of attention or judgment to the perceptual experience itself. This paper argues for the Merleau-Pontyian idea that opportunities (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  47.  43
    Motor Area Activity During Mental Rotation Studied by Time-Resolved Single-Trial fMRI.Wolfgang Richter, Randy Summers, Seong-Gi Kim & Carola Tegeler - unknown
    & The functional equivalence of overt movements and dynamic imagery is of fundamental importance in neuroscience. Here, we investigated the participation of the neocortical motor areas in a classic task of dynamic imagery, Shepard and Metzler's mental rotation task, by time-resolved single-trial functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI). The subjects performed the mental-rotation task 16 times, each time with different object pairs. Functional images were acquired for each pair separately, and the onset times and..
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  48.  35
    Motor representation in acting together.Corrado Sinigaglia & Stephen A. Butterfill - 2022 - Synthese 200 (2):1-16.
    People walk, build, paint and otherwise act together with a purpose in myriad ways. What is the relation between the actions people perform in acting together with a purpose and the outcome, or outcomes, to which their actions are directed? We argue that fully characterising this relation will require appeal not only to intention, knowledge and other familiar philosophical paraphernalia but also to another kind of representation involved in preparing and executing actions, namely motor representation. If we are right, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  49.  33
    Motor processes in mental rotation.Mark Wexler, Stephen M. Kosslyn & Alain Berthoz - 1998 - Cognition 68 (1):77-94.
    Much indirect evidence supports the hypothesis that transformations of mental images are at least in part guided by motor processes, even in the case of images of abstract objects rather than of body parts. For example, rotation may be guided by processes that also prime one to see results of a specific motor action. We directly test the hypothesis by means of a dual-task paradigm in which subjects perform the Cooper-Shepard mental rotation task while executing an unseen (...) rotation in a given direction and at a previously learned speed. Four results support the inference that mental rotation relies on motor processes. First, motor rotation that is compatible with mental rotation results in faster times and fewer errors in the imagery task than when the two rotations are incompatible. Second, the angle through which subjects rotate their mental images, and the angle through which they rotate a joystick handle are correlated, but only if the directions of the two rotations are compatible. Third, motor rotation modifies the classical inverted V-shaped mental rotation response time function, favoring the direction of the motor rotation; indeed, in some cases motor rotation even shifts the location of the minimum of this curve in the direction of the motor rotation. Fourth, the preceding effect is sensitive not only to the direction of the motor rotation, but also to the motor speed. A change in the speed of motor rotation can correspondingly slow down or speed up the mental rotation. (shrink)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   60 citations  
  50.  11
    Motor Imagery and Action Observation as Appropriate Strategies for Home-Based Rehabilitation: A Mini-Review Focusing on Improving Physical Function in Orthopedic Patients.Armin H. Paravlic - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Dynamic stability of the knee and weakness of the extensor muscles are considered to be the most important functional limitations after anterior cruciate ligament injury, probably due to changes at the central level of motor control rather than at the peripheral level. Despite general technological advances, fewer contraindicative surgical procedures, and extensive postoperative rehabilitation, up to 65% of patients fail to return to their preinjury level of sports, and only half were able to return to competitive sport. Later, it (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000