Results for ' interval length'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  10
    Intertrial interval length and discrimination learning in young chicks.Bruce A. Mattingly & James F. Zolman - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 13 (5):314-316.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  14
    Effects of interstimulus interval length and variability on habituation of autonomic components of the orienting response.Robert J. Gatchel & Peter J. Lang - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (4):802.
  3.  24
    Interactive effects on reaction time of preparatory interval length and preparatory interval frequency.Alfred A. Baumeister & Charles E. Joubert - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (2):393.
  4. Determinants of birth interval length.James Trussell, Barbara Vaughan, Samir Farid, T. Kanitkar, B. N. Murthy, M. M. Gandotra, N. Das, V. Fuster, A. K. Majumder & S. H. Lee - 1989 - Journal of Biosocial Science 21 (4):133-58.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  15
    The a-b, b-c, a-c mediation paradigm: The effects of variation in a-c study- and test-interval lengths and strength of a-b or b-c.Rudolph W. Schulz & George E. Weaver - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (2p1):291.
  6.  8
    Length of time interval in successive association.Harvey Carr - 1919 - Psychological Review 26 (5):335-353.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  12
    Variable-interval and fixed-interval schedule preferences in pigeons as a function of signaled reinforcement and schedule length.Sandra M. Schrader & Howard Rachlin - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8 (6):445-448.
  8.  5
    Effect of interstimulus intervals and rest-period length upon habituation of the orienting response.James H. Geer - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (4):617.
  9.  17
    Pronunciation and the length of the study interval in verbal discrimination.Ronald H. Hopkins & William F. Epling - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 88 (1):145.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10.  18
    Delayed partial reinforcement: Length of delay interval and the relationship of independence.Steven J. Haggbloom & E. J. Capaldi - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12 (6):457-460.
  11.  18
    The relationship between length of interval separating two learning tasks and performance on the second task.Charles E. Hamilton - 1950 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 40 (5):613.
  12.  19
    Effects of delay of informative feedback and length of postfeedback interval on concept identification.Lyle E. Bourne & C. Victor Bunderson - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (1):1.
  13.  20
    Effects of evaluation apprehension on memory over intervals of varying lengths.Russell G. Green - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (5):908.
  14.  20
    Experimental studies of rhythm and time: II. The preferred length of interval (tempo).J. E. Wallace Wallin - 1911 - Psychological Review 18 (3):202-222.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  21
    Effect of amount of interpolated learning and length of retention interval upon retroactive inhibition in a serial search task.Robert E. Hicks & Robert K. Young - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 100 (2):297.
  16.  14
    Remote associations as a function of the length of interval between learning and recall.J. T. Wilson - 1943 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 33 (1):40.
  17.  26
    When one’s sense of agency goes wrong: Absent modulation of time perception by voluntary actions and reduction of perceived length of intervals in passivity symptoms in schizophrenia.Kyran T. Graham-Schmidt, Mathew T. Martin-Iverson, Nicholas P. Holmes & Flavie A. V. Waters - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 45:9-23.
  18.  11
    Short-term retention of sequentially presented digits as a function of interdigit interval, digit duration, and series length.Thomas E. Sitterley - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 78 (1):174.
  19.  16
    Concept identification: The effects of varying length and informational components of the intertrial interval.Lyle E. Bourne, Donald E. Guy, David H. Dodd & Don R. Justeen - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (6):624.
  20.  14
    Effect of amount and distribution of inspection time and length of decay interval on kinesthetic after-effect.Jean B. Carlson - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (4):377.
  21.  17
    Effects of formal interitem similarity and length of retention interval on proactive inhibition of short-term memory.John H. Wright - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (3):386.
  22.  7
    Effects of reinforcement intervals in paired-associate learning.L. Keller, W. J. Thomson & J. R. Tweedy - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (2):268.
  23.  16
    Length Neutrosophic Subalgebras of BCK=BCI-Algebras.Young Bae Jun, Madad Khan, Florentin Smarandache & Seok-Zun Song - 2020 - Bulletin of the Section of Logic 49 (4):377-400.
    Given i, j, k ∈ {1,2,3,4}, the notion of -length neutrosophic subalgebras in BCK=BCI-algebras is introduced, and their properties are investigated. Characterizations of length neutrosophic subalgebras are discussed by using level sets of interval neutrosophic sets. Conditions for level sets of interval neutrosophic sets to be subalgebras are provided.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  24.  22
    Chains and antichains in interval algebras.M. Bekkali - 1994 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 59 (3):860-867.
    Let κ be a regular cardinal, and let B be a subalgebra of an interval algebra of size κ. The existence of a chain or an antichain of size κ in B is due to M. Rubin (see [7]). We show that if the density of B is countable, then the same conclusion holds without this assumption on κ. Next we also show that this is the best possible result by showing that it is consistent with 2 ℵ 0 (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  18
    The temporal indifference interval determined by the method of mean error.H. Woodrow - 1934 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 17 (2):167.
  26.  6
    The new moon interval NA and the beginning of the Babylonian month.John Steele - forthcoming - Archive for History of Exact Sciences:1-26.
    This study examines Babylonian records of the new moon interval NA (sunset to moonset on the day of first lunar visibility) and the connection of this interval to the length of the moon. I show that the NA intervals in the Normal Star Almanacs were computed using the goal-year method and were then used in turn to predict the lengths of each month of the year. I further argue that these predicted month lengths, adjusted occasionally on the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  10
    Effect of intertrial interval duration on component processes in concept learning.Herbert Wells - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 94 (1):49.
  28.  34
    Effects of frequency of presentation and stimulus length on retention in the Brown-Peterson paradigm.Alfred H. Fuchs & Arthur W. Melton - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (4):629.
  29.  15
    Retention of paired associates as a function of list length.Ruth Hipple - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (3):435.
  30.  27
    Semantic encoding and recognition memory: A test of encoding variability theory.Eugene Winograd & Mary F. Geis - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (6):1061.
  31. Two solutions to Galton's problem.Raoul Naroll - 1961 - Philosophy of Science 28 (1):15-39.
    Two solutions are offered to the problem of distinguishing "historical" from "functional" associations in cross-cultural surveys. The underlying logic of the mathematical model is discussed and three kinds of association distinguished: hyperdiffusional or purely "historical" association, undiffusional or purely "functional" association, and semidiffusional or mixed "historical-functional" association. Two overland diffusion arcs constitute the test sample; the relationship of social stratification to political complexity constitutes the test problem. A sifting test establishes a bimodal distribution of interval lengths between like types (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  32.  19
    Infant and child mortality determinants in Bangladesh: are they changing?Abul Kashem Majumder, Marian May & Prakash Dev Pant - 1997 - Journal of Biosocial Science 29 (4):385-399.
    From the data of the 1989 Bangladesh Fertility Survey, aggregate deaths reported at ages 0-12 and 13-60 months are used to estimate infant and child mortality. Multivariate analysis shows that preceding birth interval length, followed by survival status of the immediately preceding child, are the most important factors associated with differential infant and child mortality risks; sex of the index child and mother's and father's education are also significant. Demographic factors are influential during infancy as well as childhood, (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  33.  8
    Tracking Proactive Interference in Visual Memory.Tom Mercer, Ruby-Jane Jarvis, Rebekah Lawton & Frankie Walters - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    The current contents of visual working memory can be disrupted by previously formed memories. This phenomenon is known as proactive interference, and it can be used to index the availability of old memories. However, there is uncertainty about the robustness and lifetime of proactive interference, which raises important questions about the role of temporal factors in forgetting. The present study assessed different factors that were expected to influence the persistence of proactive interference over an inter-trial interval in the visual (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  15
    Maintenance of interference in short-term memory.Judith Goggin & Donald A. Riley - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (6):1027.
  35.  82
    A Consistent Conception of the Extended Linear Continuum as an Aggregate of Unextended Elements.Adolf Grünbaum - 1952 - Philosophy of Science 19 (4):288 - 306.
    It is a commonplace in the analytic geometry of physical space-time that an extended straight line segment, having positive length, is treated as “consisting of” unextended points, each of which has zero length. Analogously, time intervals of positive duration are resolved into instants, each of which has zero duration.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  36.  12
    Kinesthetic retention, movement extent, and information processing.George E. Stelmach & Mark Wilson - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 85 (3):425.
  37.  7
    Repetitive Control Scheme of Robotic Manipulators Based on Improved B-Spline Function.Xingyu Wang, Anna Wang, Dazhi Wang, Wenhui Wang, Bingxue Liang & Yufei Qi - 2021 - Complexity 2021:1-15.
    In this paper, a repetitive control scheme of a 2-DOF robotic manipulator based on the improved cubic B-spline curve is proposed. Firstly, a repetitive controller for robotic manipulator is designed, which is composed of an iterative controller and disturbance observer. Then, an improved B-spline optimization scheme is introduced to divide the task of the robotic manipulator into three intervals. A correction function is added to each interval of cubic spline interpolation. Finally, a variety of cases are designed and simulated (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38. Three concepts of decidability for general subsets of uncountable spaces.Matthew W. Parker - 2003 - Theoretical Computer Science 351 (1):2-13.
    There is no uniquely standard concept of an effectively decidable set of real numbers or real n-tuples. Here we consider three notions: decidability up to measure zero [M.W. Parker, Undecidability in Rn: Riddled basins, the KAM tori, and the stability of the solar system, Phil. Sci. 70(2) (2003) 359–382], which we abbreviate d.m.z.; recursive approximability [or r.a.; K.-I. Ko, Complexity Theory of Real Functions, Birkhäuser, Boston, 1991]; and decidability ignoring boundaries [d.i.b.; W.C. Myrvold, The decision problem for entanglement, in: R.S. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  39. A System of Axioms for Minkowski Spacetime.Lorenzo Cocco & Joshua Babic - 2020 - Journal of Philosophical Logic (1):1-37.
    We present an elementary system of axioms for the geometry of Minkowski spacetime. It strikes a balance between a simple and streamlined set of axioms and the attempt to give a direct formalization in first-order logic of the standard account of Minkowski spacetime in [Maudlin 2012] and [Malament, unpublished]. It is intended for future use in the formalization of physical theories in Minkowski spacetime. The choice of primitives is in the spirit of [Tarski 1959]: a predicate of betwenness and a (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  40.  52
    Which explanatory role for mathematics in scientific models? Reply to “The Explanatory Dispensability of Idealizations”.Silvia De Bianchi - 2016 - Synthese 193 (2):387-401.
    In The Explanatory Dispensability of Idealizations, Sam Baron suggests a possible strategy enabling the indispensability argument to break the symmetry between mathematical claims and idealization assumptions in scientific models. Baron’s distinction between mathematical and non-mathematical idealization, I claim, is in need of a more compelling criterion, because in scientific models idealization assumptions are expressed through mathematical claims. In this paper I argue that this mutual dependence of idealization and mathematics cannot be read in terms of symmetry and that Baron’s non-causal (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  41.  5
    A study of Babylonian planetary theory III. The planet Mercury.Teije de Jong - 2021 - Archive for History of Exact Sciences 75 (5):491-522.
    In this series of papers I attempt to provide an answer to the question how the Babylonian scholars arrived at their mathematical theory of planetary motion. Papers I and II were devoted to system A theory of the outer planets and of the planet Venus. In this third and last paper I will study system A theory of the planet Mercury. Our knowledge of the Babylonian theory of Mercury is at present based on twelveEphemeridesand sevenProcedure Texts. Three computational systems of (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42.  24
    A System of Axioms for Minkowski Spacetime.Lorenzo Cocco & Joshua Babic - 2020 - Journal of Philosophical Logic 50 (1):149-185.
    We present an elementary system of axioms for the geometry of Minkowski spacetime. It strikes a balance between a simple and streamlined set of axioms and the attempt to give a direct formalization in first-order logic of the standard account of Minkowski spacetime in Maudlin and Malament. It is intended for future use in the formalization of physical theories in Minkowski spacetime. The choice of primitives is in the spirit of Tarski : a predicate of betwenness and a four place (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  43.  71
    Chaos, History, and Narrative.George A. Reisch - 1991 - History and Theory 30 (1):1-20.
    Hempel's proposal of covering laws which explain historical events has a certain plausibility, but can never be actually realized due to the chaotic nature of history. The natural laws that would govern both individual lives and greater history would be nonlinear; consequently, in the terminology of chaos theory, the final states of both are extremely sensitive to initial conditions. Initial conditions would need to be exactly known in order to account correctly for historic phenomena, especially for causes and effects which (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  44.  79
    On the beginning of time.Quentin Smith - 1985 - Noûs 19 (4):579-584.
    You can search this site: Note that this analysis of a beginning of time concerns intervals ’of the same length' ; if this qualifying phrase is not added, then the analysis would be invalid for a dense time. If time is dense and began, then for each interval of time there is another interval of a shorter length that is a part of that interval and which completely elapses before the interval of which it (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  45.  42
    The Extent of Dilation of Sets of Probabilities and the Asymptotics of Robust Bayesian Inference.Timothy Herron, Teddy Seidenfeld & Larry Wasserman - 1994 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1994:250 - 259.
    We report two issues concerning diverging sets of Bayesian (conditional) probabilities-divergence of "posteriors"-that can result with increasing evidence. Consider a set P of probabilities typically, but not always, based on a set of Bayesian "priors." Fix E, an event of interest, and X, a random variable to be observed. With respect to P, when the set of conditional probabilities for E, given X, strictly contains the set of unconditional probabilities for E, for each possible outcome X = x, call this (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  46.  16
    Which explanatory role for mathematics in scientific models? Reply to “The Explanatory Dispensability of Idealizations”.Silvia Bianchi - 2016 - Synthese 193 (2):387-401.
    In The Explanatory Dispensability of Idealizations, Sam Baron suggests a possible strategy enabling the indispensability argument to break the symmetry between mathematical claims and idealization assumptions in scientific models. Baron’s distinction between mathematical and non-mathematical idealization, I claim, is in need of a more compelling criterion, because in scientific models idealization assumptions are expressed through mathematical claims. In this paper I argue that this mutual dependence of idealization and mathematics cannot be read in terms of symmetry and that Baron’s non-causal (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  47.  26
    From telluric helix to telluric remix.Philip J. Stewart - 2019 - Foundations of Chemistry 22 (1):3-14.
    The first attempt to represent the Periodic system graphically was the Telluric Helix presented in 1862 by Alexandre-Emile Béguyer de Chancourtois, in which the sequence of elements was wound round a cylinder. This has hardly been attempted since, because the intervals between periodic returns vary in length from 2 to 32 elements, but Charles Janet presented a model wound round four nested cylinders. The rows in Janet’s table are defined by a constant sum of the first two quantum numbers, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  48.  30
    A Molecular Logic of Chords and Their Internal Harmony.Ingolf Max - 2018 - Logica Universalis 12 (1-2):239-269.
    Chords are not pure sets of tones or notes. They are mainly characterized by their matrices. A chord matrix is the pattern of all the lengths of intervals given without further context. Chords are well-structured invariants. They show their inner logical form. This opens up the possibility to develop a molecular logic of chords. Chords are our primitive, but, nevertheless, already interrelated expressions. The logical space of internal harmony is our well-known chromatic scale represented by an infinite line of integers. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  49.  41
    Precision in theory and in measurement.Joseph Agassi - 1968 - Philosophy of Science 35 (3):287-290.
    An intuitive idea concerning degrees of precision is widely accepted, and it is that we increase precision of theories by paying attention to ever decreasing orders of magnitude of measurements which we incorporate in these theories. We increase precision of measuring or of predicting measurement of length, for instance, if we pay attention not only to centimeters but also to millimeters, microns, angstroms, and so on. And our theories are precise to centimeters, then to millimeters, and so on respectively. (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  50.  20
    Temporal dysfunction in traumatic brain injury patients: primary or secondary impairment?Giovanna Mioni, Simon Grondin & Franca Stablum - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:87074.
    Adequate temporal abilities are required for most daily activities. Traumatic brain injury (TBI) patients often present with cognitive dysfunctions, but few studies have investigated temporal impairments associated with TBI. The aim of the present work is to review the existing literature on temporal abilities in TBI patients. Particular attention is given to the involvement of higher cognitive processes in temporal processing in order to determine if any temporal dysfunction observed in TBI patients is due to the disruption of an internal (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 1000