Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The dark side of hegemony.Charles Locurto - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):153-154.
  • Extending the “new hegemony” of classical conditioning.Dan Lloyd - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-153.
  • A cerebellar long-term depression update.David J. Linden - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):482-487.
  • On learnability, empirical foundations, and naturalness.W. J. M. Levelt - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):501-501.
  • Connectionism and motivation are compatible.Daniel S. Levine - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):487-487.
  • The notions of joint stiffness and synaptic plasticity in motor memory.Lev P. Latash & Mark L. Latash - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):465-466.
    We criticize the synaptic theory of long-term memory and the inappropriate usage of physical notions such as in motor control theories. Motor control and motor memory hypotheses should be based on explicitly specified hypothetical control variables that are sound from both physiological and physical perspectives. [HOUK et al.; SMITH; THACH].
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Generality and applications.Jill H. Larkin - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):486-487.
  • Approaches to learning and representation.Pat Langley - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):500-501.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What can psychologists learn from hidden-unit nets?K. Lamberts & G. D'Ydewalle - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):499-500.
  • Classical conditioning beyond the laboratory.Hugh Lacey - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):152-152.
  • How connectionist models learn: The course of learning in connectionist networks.John K. Kruschke - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):498-499.
  • Pavlovian conditioning: Providing a bridge between cognition and biology.Marvin D. Krank - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):151-151.
  • Beyond respondent conditioning.Sibylle Klosterhalfen & Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):149-150.
  • A promising new strategy for studying conditioned Immunomodulation.Wolfgang Klosterhalfen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):150-150.
  • Underestimating the importance of the implementational level.Michael Van Kleeck - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):497-498.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The importance of classical conditioning.H. D. Kimmel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):148-149.
  • Complexity at the organismic and neuronal levels.R. W. Kentridge - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-148.
  • Associative theory versus classical conditioning: Their proper relationship.E. James Kehoe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):147-147.
  • A bridge between cerebellar long-term depression and discrete motor learning: Studies on gene knockout mice.Masanobu Kano - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):488-490.
  • A non-empiricist perspective on learning in layered networks.Michael I. Jordan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):497-498.
  • What is classical conditioning?W. J. Jacobs - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):146-146.
  • Do current connectionist learning models account for reading development in different languages?Florian Hutzler, Johannes C. Ziegler, Conrad Perry, Heinz Wimmer & Marco Zorzi - 2004 - Cognition 91 (3):273-296.
  • More models of the cerebellum.James C. Houk & Andrew G. Barto - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):492-496.
  • Models of the cerebellum and motor learning.James C. Houk, Jay T. Buckingham & Andrew G. Barto - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):368-383.
    This article reviews models of the cerebellum and motor learning, from the landmark papers by Marr and Albus through those of the present time. The unique architecture of the cerebellar cortex is ideally suited for pattern recognition, but how is pattern recognition incorporated into motor control and learning systems? The present analysis begins with a discussion of exactly what the cerebellar cortex needs to regulate through its anatomically defined projections to premotor networks. Next, we examine various models showing how the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Cerebellar arm ataxia: Theories still have a lot to explain.J. Hore - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):457.
  • Preparatory response hypotheses: A muddle of causal and functional analyses.Karen L. Hollis - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):145-146.
  • Positive cerebellar feedback loops.Germund Hesslow - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):455-456.
  • But what is the substance of connectionist representation?James Hendler - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):496-497.
  • A flawed analogy?James Hendler - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):485-486.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Two separate pathways for cerebellar LTD: NO-dependent and NO-independent.Nick A. Hartell - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):453-455.
  • Learning mechanisms in cue reweighting.Zara Harmon, Kaori Idemaru & Vsevolod Kapatsinski - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):76-88.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • What connectionist models learn: Learning and representation in connectionist networks.Stephen José Hanson & David J. Burr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):471-489.
    Connectionist models provide a promising alternative to the traditional computational approach that has for several decades dominated cognitive science and artificial intelligence, although the nature of connectionist models and their relation to symbol processing remains controversial. Connectionist models can be characterized by three general computational features: distinct layers of interconnected units, recursive rules for updating the strengths of the connections during learning, and “simple” homogeneous computing elements. Using just these three features one can construct surprisingly elegant and powerful models of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • Learning and representation: Tensions at the interface.Steven José Hanson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):511-518.
  • Expose hidden assumptions in network theory.Karl Haberlandt - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):495-496.
  • Direct Associations or Internal Transformations? Exploring the Mechanisms Underlying Sequential Learning Behavior.Todd M. Gureckis & Bradley C. Love - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (1):10-50.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Theoretical and computational analysis of skill learning, repetition priming, and procedural memory.Prahlad Gupta & Neal J. Cohen - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (2):401-448.
  • Classical conditioning: The role of interdisciplinary theory.Stephen Grossberg - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):144-145.
  • Connectionism in Pavlovian harness.George Graham - 1987 - Southern Journal of Philosophy (Suppl.) 73 (S1):73-91.
  • Connectionism in Pavlovian harness.George Graham - 1991 - In Terence E. Horgan & John L. Tienson (eds.), Connectionism and the Philosophy of Mind. Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 143--166.
  • Connectionism in Pavlovian Harness.George Graham - 1988 - Southern Journal of Philosophy 26 (S1):73-91.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning to divide the labor: an account of deficits in light and heavy verb production.Jean K. Gordon & Gary S. Dell - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (1):1-40.
    Theories of sentence production that involve a convergence of activation from conceptual‐semantic and syntactic‐sequential units inspired a connectionist model that was trained to produce simple sentences. The model used a learning algorithm that resulted in a sharing of responsibility (or “division of labor”) between syntactic and semantic inputs for lexical activation according to their predictive power. Semantically rich, or “heavy”, verbs in the model came to rely on semantic cues more than on syntactic cues, whereas semantically impoverished, or “light”, verbs (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Ambiguities in “the algorithmic level”.Alvin I. Goldman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):484-485.
  • Are connectionist models just statistical pattern classifiers?Richard M. Golden - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):494-495.
  • The study of cognition and instructional design: Mutual nurturance.Robert Glaser - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):483-484.
  • How and what does the cerebellum learn?Peter F. C. Gilbert - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):449-450.
  • Cerebellum does more than recalibration of movements after perturbations.C. Gielen - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):448-449.
    We argue that the function of the cerebellum is more than just an error-detecting mechanism. Rather, the cerebellum plays an important role in all movements. The bias in (re)calibration is an unfortunate restrictive result of a very successful and important experiment, [SMITH, THACH].
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond Pavlovian classical conditioning.Beatrix T. Gardner & R. Allen Gardner - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):143-144.
  • Flights of teleological fancy about classical conditioning do not produce valid science or useful technology.John J. Furedy - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):142-143.
  • The Fallacy of the Homuncular Fallacy.Carrie Figdor - 2018 - Belgrade Philosophical Annual 31:41-56.
    A leading theoretical framework for naturalistic explanation of mind holds that we explain the mind by positing progressively "stupider" capacities ("homunculi") until the mind is "discharged" by means of capacities that are not intelligent at all. The so-called homuncular fallacy involves violating this procedure by positing the same capacities at subpersonal levels. I argue that the homuncular fallacy is not a fallacy, and that modern-day homunculi are idle posits. I propose an alternative view of what naturalism requires that reflects how (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Metacognitive Myopia in Hidden-Profile Tasks: The Failure to Control for Repetition Biases.Klaus Fiedler, Joscha Hofferbert & Franz Wöllert - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark