Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Testing Mealey's model: The need to demonstrate an ESS and to establish the role of testosterone.John Archer - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):541-542.
    Two specific aspects of Mealey's model are questioned: (1) the application of the concept of Evolutionarily Stable Strategy to all alternative strategies, including those that involve reduced lifetime reproductive success; and (2) the evidence for the dual role of testosterone, which is based mainly on studies of a modulating effect on aggression.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Contiguity, contingency, and causation.R. J. Andrew - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):447.
  • Cognitive algebra versus representativeness heuristic.Norman H. Anderson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):17-17.
    Cognitive algebra strongly disproved the representativeness heuristic almost before it was published; and therewith it also disproved the base rate fallacy. Cognitive algebra provides a theoretical foundation for judgment-decision theory through its joint solution to the two fundamental problems – true measurement of subjective values, and cognitive rules for integration of multiple determinants.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skinner's circus.Stuart A. Altmann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):678-679.
  • Principles of learning and the ecological style of inquiry.Thomas R. Alley & Robert E. Shaw - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):139-141.
  • Instrumental and contingent saccharin-licking in rats: Response deprivation and reinforcement.James Allison & William Timberlake - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 2 (3):141-143.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Defense motivational system: Issues of emotion, reinforcement, and neural structure.David Adams - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):675-676.
  • Reconsidering linguistic nativism from an interdisciplinary, emergentist perspective.Michael Breyl - 2023 - Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 5 (2):162-193.
    For decades, interdisciplinary research efforts have accumulated insights that diminish the significance of the classic nature versus nurture dichotomy, instead calling for a nuanced, multifactorial approach to ontogeny. Similarly, the role of genes in both phylogeny and ontogeny, once seen as rather deterministic, is now conceptualized as highly dependent on environmental factors, including behavior. Linguistic theories have, in principle, made an effort to incorporate these changing views. However, the central claim of the given paper is that this apparent compliance with (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modularity and the Politics of Emotion Categorisation.Raamy Majeed - 2022 - A Tribute to Ronald de Sousa.
    Empirically-informed approaches to emotion often construe our emotions as modules: systems hardwired into our brains by evolution and purpose-built to generate certain coordinated patterns of expressive, physiological, behavioural and phenomenological responses. In ‘Against Modularity’ (2008), de Sousa argues that we shouldn’t think of our emotions in terms of a limited number of modules because this conflicts with our aspirations for a life of greater emotional richness. My aim in this paper is to defend de Sousa’s critique of modular emotion taxonomies (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Reliability and Validity of Experiment in the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.Sullivan Jacqueline Anne - 2007 - Dissertation, University of Pittsburgh
  • On the what_ and _how of learning.R. C. Gonzalez & Matthew Yarczower - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):145-145.
  • Contrasting approaches to a theory of learning.Timothy D. Johnston - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):125-139.
    The general process view of learning, which guided research into learning for the first half of this century, has come under attack in recent years from several quarters. One form of criticism has come from proponents of the so-called biological boundaries approach to learning. These theorists have presented a variety of data showing that supposedly general laws of learning may in fact be limited in their applicability to different species and learning tasks, and they argue that the limitations are drawn (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   287 citations  
  • The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Phylogenic and ontogenic environments.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):701-711.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Neuropsychology vis-à-vis Skinner's behaviouristic psychology.Gerhard D. Wassermann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):700-701.
  • Each behavior is a product of heredity and experience.Douglas Wahlsten - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):699-700.
  • Reinforcement is the problem, not the solution: Variation and selection of behavior.J. E. R. Staddon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):697-699.
  • Skinner's practical metaphysic may be impractical.S. N. Salthe - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):696-697.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is evolution of behavior operant conditioning writ large?Anatol Rapoport - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):696-696.
  • Nature and nurture revisited.H. C. Plotkin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):695-696.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hereditary ≠ innate.Robert Plomin & Denise Daniels - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):694-695.
  • B. F. Skinner and the flaws of sociobiology.Anthony J. Perzigian - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):693-694.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Molar concepts and mentalistic theories: A moral perspective.Stephen Kaplan - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):692-693.
  • The use of evolutionary analogies and the rejection of state variables by B. F. Skinner.Alejandro Kacelnik & Alasdair Houston - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):691-692.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavior in the light of identified neurons.Graham Hoyle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):690-691.
  • The structure versus the provenance of behavior.Jerry A. Hogan - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):690-690.
  • Ethology ignored Skinner to its detriment.Jack P. Hailman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):689-690.
  • Lingering Haeckelian influences and certain other inadequacies of the operant viewpoint for phylogeny and ontogeny.Gilbert Gottlieb - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):688-689.
  • B. F. Skinner versus Dr. Pangloss.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):687-688.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skinner's blind eye.H. J. Eysenck - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):686-687.
  • Difficulties with phylogenetic and ontogenetic concepts.Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):685-686.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consequence contingencies and provenance partitions.Juan D. Delius - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):685-685.
  • Operant conditioning and natural selection.Andrew M. Colman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):684-685.
  • Ethology and operant psychology.Gordon M. Burghardt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):683-684.
  • Cost–benefit models and the evolution of behavior.Jerram L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):682-682.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A new experimental analysis of behavior – one for all behavior.D. Caroline Blanchard, Robert J. Blanchard & Kevin J. Flannelly - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):681-682.
  • Of false dichotomies and larger frames.Jerome H. Barkow - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):680-681.
  • Contingencies of selection, reinforcement, and survival.David P. Barash - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):680-680.
  • Ontogenetic or phylogenetic – another afterpain of the fallacious Cartesian dichotomy.Gerard P. Baerends - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):679-680.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):669-677.
    Responses are strengthened by consequences having to do with the survival of individuals and species. With respect to the provenance of behavior, we know more about ontogenic than phylogenic contingencies. The contingencies responsible for unlearned behavior acted long ago. This remoteness affects our scientific methods, both experimental and conceptual. Until we have identified he variables responsible for an event, we tend to invent causes. Explanatory entities such as “instincts,” “drives,” and “traits” still survive. Unable to show how organisms can behave (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The uneven distribution of fears and phobias: A nonassociative account.Ross G. Menzies - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):305-306.
    A review of data concerning the uneven distribution of phobias suggests that nonassociative, ethological models can account for most of tile important findings that cannot be attributed to expectancy biases. The origin of a variety of fears that appear in fixed developmental patterns across divergent cultures and species can best be explained by biological models.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preparedness, phobias, and the Panglossian paradigm.Richard J. McNally - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):303-304.
    In his critique of preparedness theory, Davey does not address the limitations of adaptationism. The purpose of this commentary is to outline problems that arise when one assumes that mental illness (e.g., phobic disorder)musthave had adaptive significance for it to have survived the vicissitudes of natural selection.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Preparedness and phobias: Specific evolved associations or a generalized expectancy bias?Graham C. L. Davey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):289-297.
    Most phobias are focussed on a small number of fear-inducing stimuli (e.g., snakes, spiders). A review of the evidence supporting biological and cognitive explanations of this uneven distribution of phobias suggests that the readiness with which such stimuli become associated with aversive outcomes arises from biases in the processing of information about threatening stimuli rather than from phylogenetically based associative predispositions or “biological preparedness.” This cognitive bias, consisting of a heightened expectation of aversive outcomes following fear-relevant stimuli, generates and maintains (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Issues for the next generation of base rate research.Jonathan J. Koehler - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):41-53.
    Commentators agree that simple conclusions about a general base rate fallacy are not appropriate. It is more constructive to identify conditions under which base rates are differentially weighted. Commentators also agree that improving the ecological validity of the research is desirable, although this is less important to those interested exclusively in psychological processes. The philosophers and ecologists among the commentators offer a kinder perspective on base rate reasoning than the psychologists. My own perspective is that the interesting questions (both psychological (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The base rate fallacy reconsidered: Descriptive, normative, and methodological challenges.Jonathan J. Koehler - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):1-17.
    We have been oversold on the base rate fallacy in probabilistic judgment from an empirical, normative, and methodological standpoint. At the empirical level, a thorough examination of the base rate literature (including the famous lawyer–engineer problem) does not support the conventional wisdom that people routinely ignore base rates. Quite the contrary, the literature shows that base rates are almost always used and that their degree of use depends on task structure and representation. Specifically, base rates play a relatively larger role (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   84 citations  
  • Base rates do not constrain nonprobability judgments.Paul D. Windschitl & Gary L. Wells - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):40-41.
    Base rates have no necessary relation to judgments that are not themselves probabilities. There is no logical imperative, for instance, that behavioral base rates must affect causal attributions or that base rate information should affect judgments of legal liability. Decision theorists should be cautious in arguing that base rates place normative constraints on judgments of anything other than posterior probabilities.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The perils of reconstructive remembering and the value of representative design.Kim J. Vicente - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):40-40.
    Abstract(1) The miscitations of seminal experiments in the base rate literature adds to the existing database of systematic miscitations of wellknown psychological experiments. These miscitations may be caused by a process of reconstructive remembering. (2) Representative design should be the methodological core of Koehler's call for ecologically valid research. This approach can benefit both basic and applied research.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Throwing out the baby with the bathwater? Let's not overstate the overselling of the base rate fallacy.Cynthia J. Thomsen & Eugene Borgida - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):39-40.
    Koehler's summary and critique of research on the base rate fallacy is cogent and persuasive. However, he may have overstated the case, and his suggestions for future research may be too restrictive. We agree that methodological approaches to this topic should be broadened, but we argue that experimental laboratory research and the Bayesian normative standard are useful and should not be abandoned.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Where do you stand on the base rate issue?Douglas Stalker - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):38-39.
    This commentary presents a self-assessment inventory that will allow readers to determine their own attitude toward the base rate fallacy and its literature. The inventory is scientifically valid but not Medicare/Medicaid reimbursable.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The implicit use of base rates in experiential and ecologically valid tasks.Barbara A. Spellman - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):38-38.
    When base rates are learned and used in an experiential manner subjects show better base rate use, perhaps because the implicit learning system is engaged. A causal framework in which base rates are relevant might also be necessary. Humans might thus perform better on more ecologically valid tasks, which are likely to contain those three components.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation