Results for 'simple learning'

988 found
Order:
  1.  76
    A mathematical model for simple learning.Robert R. Bush & Frederick Mosteller - 1951 - Psychological Review 58 (5):313-323.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  2.  18
    A study of simple learning under irrelevant motivational-reward conditions.K. W. Spence, G. Bergmann & R. Lippitt - 1950 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 40 (5):539.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3. Foundations of Statistical Learning Theory, 1. The Linear Model for Simple Learning.W. K. Estes & Patrick Suppes - 1959 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 10 (39):251-252.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  4.  13
    Subcortical mechanisms in learning: I. The functional significance of subcortical nuclei in certain simple learning tasks, with a description of a program for further experimental work.C. W. Brown - 1935 - Psychological Review 42 (4):307-334.
  5.  10
    Some criticisms of "A mathematical model for simple learning.".Raymond H. Burros - 1952 - Psychological Review 59 (3):234-236.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  15
    Learning Simple Spatial Terms: Core and More.Barbara Landau - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):91-114.
    Landau also pushes the role of syntax and its mapping to semantics in learning what some would consider “easy words”—the simplest spatial prepositions in English, in and on. Taking as a starting point that the syntactic distribution of a word is a reflex of its meaning, Landau shows that careful study of how children and adults linguistically encode a range of containment and support configurations reveals a special status for “core” configurations in each domain. She proposes that children come (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  7.  24
    Learning of simple structures.George Mandler & Philip A. Cowan - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (2):177.
  8.  16
    Learning Simple Things: A Connectionist Learning Problem from Various Perspectives.Edward P. Stabler - 1988 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1988:424 - 441.
    The performance of a connectionist learning system on a simple problem has been described by Hinton and is briefly reviewed here: a finite set is learned from a finite collection of finite sets, and the system generalizes correctly from partial information by finding simple "features" of the environment. For comparison, a very similar problem is formulated in the Gold paradigm of discrete learning functions. To get generalization similar to the connectionist system, a non-conservative learning strategy (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9. Simple heuristics from the adaptive toolbox: Can we perform the requisite learning?Dr Tim Rakow, Neal Hinvest, Edward Jackson & Martin Palmer - 2004 - Thinking and Reasoning 10 (1):1 – 29.
    The Adaptive Toolbox framework specifies heuristics for choice and categorisation that search through cues in previously learned orders (Gigerenzer & Todd, 1999). We examined the learning of three cue parameters defining different orders: discrimination rate (DR) (the probability that a cue points to a unique choice), validity (the probability of correct choice given that a cue discriminates), and success (the probability of correct choice). Success orderings are identical to those by expected information gain (Klayman & Ha, 1987). In two (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  10.  1
    Discovering simple rules in complex data: A meta-learning algorithm and some surprising musical discoveries.Gerhard Widmer - 2003 - Artificial Intelligence 146 (2):129-148.
  11.  31
    Learning from Simple Indicative Conditionals.Leendert M. Huisman - 2017 - Erkenntnis 82 (3):583-601.
    An agent who receives information in the form of an indicative conditional statement and who trusts her source will modify her credences to bring them in line with the conditional. I will argue that the agent, upon the acquisition of such information, should, in general, expand her prior credence function to an indeterminate posterior one; that is, to a set of credence functions. Two different ways the agent might interpret the conditional will be presented, and the properties of the resulting (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  12.  11
    A Dual Simple Recurrent Network Model for Chunking and Abstract Processes in Sequence Learning.Lituan Wang, Yangqin Feng, Qiufang Fu, Jianyong Wang, Xunwei Sun, Xiaolan Fu, Lei Zhang & Zhang Yi - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Although many studies have provided evidence that abstract knowledge can be acquired in artificial grammar learning, it remains unclear how abstract knowledge can be attained in sequence learning. To address this issue, we proposed a dual simple recurrent network model that includes a surface SRN encoding and predicting the surface properties of stimuli and an abstract SRN encoding and predicting the abstract properties of stimuli. The results of Simulations 1 and 2 showed that the DSRN model can (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  22
    Learning in simple systems.Geoffrey Hall - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (2):210-211.
    Studies of conditioning in simple systems are best interpreted in terms of the formation of excitatory links. The mechanisms responsible for such conditioning contribute to the associative learning effects shown by more complex systems. If a dual-system approach is to be avoided, the best hope lies in developing standard associative theory to deal with phenomena said to show propositional learning.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14.  22
    Simple heuristics for enhancing GP learning.J. R. Villar, E. A. de la Cal, J. Sedano & M. A. Garcia Tamargo - 2015 - Logic Journal of the IGPL 23 (3):472-484.
  15.  23
    Learning of a simple grapho-motor task by young children and adults: similar acquisition but age-dependent retention.Mona S. Julius & Esther Adi-Japha - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  16.  6
    Simple conditioning as two-stage all-or-none learning.John Theios - 1963 - Psychological Review 70 (5):403-417.
  17.  17
    Simple concept learning as a function of intralist generalization.Marian Hooper Baum - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (2):89.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  76
    Simple Models in Complex Worlds: Occam’s Razor and Statistical Learning Theory.Falco J. Bargagli Stoffi, Gustavo Cevolani & Giorgio Gnecco - 2022 - Minds and Machines 32 (1):13-42.
    The idea that “simplicity is a sign of truth”, and the related “Occam’s razor” principle, stating that, all other things being equal, simpler models should be preferred to more complex ones, have been long discussed in philosophy and science. We explore these ideas in the context of supervised machine learning, namely the branch of artificial intelligence that studies algorithms which balance simplicity and accuracy in order to effectively learn about the features of the underlying domain. Focusing on statistical (...) theory, we show that situations exist for which a preference for simpler models provably slows down, instead of favoring, the supervised learning process. Our results shed new light on the relations between simplicity and truth approximation, which are briefly discussed in the context of both machine learning and the philosophy of science. (shrink)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  12
    Simple trial and error learning: A study in psychological theory.C. L. Hull - 1930 - Psychological Review 37 (3):241-256.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  20.  39
    Evolution, learning, games, and simple heuristics.Peter Hammerstein - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (5):752-753.
    Humans are incapable of acting as utility maximisers. In contrast, the evolutionary process had considerable time and computational power to select optimal heuristics from a set of alternatives. To view evolution as the optimising agent has revolutionised game theory. Gigerenzer and his co-workers can help us understand the circumstances under which evolution and learning achieve optimisation and Nash equilibria.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  22
    Simple heuristics from the Adaptive Toolbox: Can we perform the requisite learning?Tim Rakow, Neal Hinvest, Edward Jackson & Martin Palmer - 2004 - Thinking and Reasoning 10 (1):1-29.
  22.  10
    Learning sets for simple concept identification and reversal shifts.Lorraine A. Low, Frederick Gronberg & Bernice Sherling - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 5 (3):254-256.
  23.  4
    Simple qualitative discrimination learning.Clark L. Hull - 1950 - Psychological Review 57 (5):303-313.
  24. Social Learning Strategies in Networked Groups.Thomas N. Wisdom, Xianfeng Song & Robert L. Goldstone - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (8):1383-1425.
    When making decisions, humans can observe many kinds of information about others' activities, but their effects on performance are not well understood. We investigated social learning strategies using a simple problem-solving task in which participants search a complex space, and each can view and imitate others' solutions. Results showed that participants combined multiple sources of information to guide learning, including payoffs of peers' solutions, popularity of solution elements among peers, similarity of peers' solutions to their own, and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  25.  39
    Learning simple and complex artificial grammars in the presence of a semantic reference field: effects on performance and awareness.Esther Van den Bos & Fenna H. Poletiek - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  11
    Children’s Learning From Interactive eBooks: Simple Irrelevant Features Are Not Necessarily Worse Than Relevant Ones.Roxanne A. Etta & Heather L. Kirkorian - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    The purpose of this study was to investigate experimentally the extent to which children’s novel word learning and story comprehension from eBooks depends on the relevance of interactive eBook features. A story was created in the lab to incorporate novel word-object pairs. The story was read to preschoolers (3-5 years old, N = 103) using one of the three books: noninteractive control, interactive-relevant, interactive-irrelevant. Novel word learning and story comprehension were assessed with posttests in which children picked target (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27.  12
    Compound and simple responses in paired-associate learning.Joseph L. Young & Robert L. Schiffer - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 91 (2):206.
  28.  21
    Interpolated activity and the learning of a simple skill.Kenneth A. Blick & Edward A. Bilodeau - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):515.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29. Neural analysis of learning in simple systems.Frank Krasne - 2002 - In J. Wixted & H. Pashler (eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30. Conditions affecting simple vocabulary learning.Jw Hall - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (5):337-337.
  31.  46
    Place versus response learning in the simple T-maze.Hugh C. Blodgett & Kenneth McCutchan - 1947 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 37 (5):412.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  39
    Reading Aloud and Solving Simple Arithmetic Calculation Intervention (Learning Therapy) Improves Inhibition, Verbal Episodic Memory, Focus Attention and Processing Speed in Healthy Elderly People: Evidence from a Randomized Controlled Trial.Rui Nouchi, Yasuyuki Taki, Hikaru Takeuchi, Takayuki Nozawa, Atsushi Sekiguchi & Ryuta Kawashima - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
  33.  21
    A model of learning for simple repeating binary patterns.Paul C. Vitz & Thomas C. Todd - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (1):108.
  34.  13
    Water descent: A simple, effective technique for avoidance learning in hamsters.M. Andrew DuBois & Kenneth B. Melvin - 1982 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 20 (4):231-232.
  35.  7
    Multi-agent learning and the descriptive value of simple models.Ido Erev & Alvin E. Roth - 2007 - Artificial Intelligence 171 (7):423-428.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  27
    No support for dual process accounts of human affective learning in simple Pavlovian conditioning.Ottmar V. Lipp & Helena M. Purkis - 2005 - Cognition and Emotion 19 (2):269-282.
    Dual process accounts of affective learning state that the learning of likes and dislikes reflects a learning mechanism that is distinct from the one reflected in expectancy learning, the learning of signal relationships, and has different empirical characteristics. Affective learning, for example, is said not to be affected by: (a) extinction training; (b) occasion setting; (c) cue competition; and (d) awareness of the CS-US contingencies. These predictions were tested in a series of experiments that (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  37.  21
    Is there a cell-biological alphabet for simple forms of learning?Robert D. Hawkins & Eric R. Kandel - 1984 - Psychological Review 91 (3):375-391.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  38.  16
    Variation of postfeedback interval in simple motor learning.Dorothe R. Weinberg, Donald E. Guy & Ronald W. Tupper - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (1):98.
  39.  72
    Generation of information and complexity: Different forms of learning and innovation: A simple mechanism of learning.Peter Schuster - 2005 - Complexity 10 (4):12-14.
  40.  8
    The role of absolute initial response strength in simple trial-and-error learning.P. S. Siegel - 1945 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 35 (3):199.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  61
    Three ideal observer models for rule learning in simple languages.Michael C. Frank & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2011 - Cognition 120 (3):360-371.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  42.  5
    High-Speed Videography Reveals How Honeybees Can Turn a Spatial Concept Learning Task Into a Simple Discrimination Task by Stereotyped Flight Movements and Sequential Inspection of Pattern Elements.Marie Guiraud, Mark Roper & Lars Chittka - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43.  34
    Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: can simple recurrent networks obviate the need for domain-specific learning devices?Gary F. Marcus - 1999 - Cognition 73 (3):293-296.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  31
    Variable frequency of knowledge of results and the learning of a simple skill.Edward A. Bilodeau & Ina McD Bilodeau - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (4):379.
  45. Understanding from Machine Learning Models.Emily Sullivan - 2022 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 73 (1):109-133.
    Simple idealized models seem to provide more understanding than opaque, complex, and hyper-realistic models. However, an increasing number of scientists are going in the opposite direction by utilizing opaque machine learning models to make predictions and draw inferences, suggesting that scientists are opting for models that have less potential for understanding. Are scientists trading understanding for some other epistemic or pragmatic good when they choose a machine learning model? Or are the assumptions behind why minimal models provide (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  46.  27
    Effect of the spatial relationship between cue, reward, and response in simple discrimination learning.J. V. Murphy & R. E. Miller - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 56 (1):26.
  47.  75
    Learning to cooperate: Reciprocity and self-control.Peter Danielson - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):256-257.
    Using a simple learning agent, we show that learning self-control in the primrose path experiment does parallel learning cooperation in the prisoner's dilemma. But Rachlin's claim that “there is no essential difference between self-control and altruism” is too strong. Only iterated prisoner's dilemmas played against reciprocators are reduced to self-control problems. There is more to cooperation than self-control and even altruism in a strong sense.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48. Cultural learning.Michael Tomasello, Ann Cale Kruger & Hilary Horn Ratner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):495-511.
    This target article presents a theory of human cultural learning. Cultural learning is identified with those instances of social learning in which intersubjectivity or perspective-taking plays a vital role, both in the original learning process and in the resulting cognitive product. Cultural learning manifests itself in three forms during human ontogeny: imitative learning, instructed learning, and collaborative learning – in that order. Evidence is provided that this progression arises from the developmental ordering (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   369 citations  
  49.  17
    The effect of using differential end boxes in a simple T-maze learning situation.M. Ray Denny - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (3):245.
  50.  28
    The effect of differential non-reinforcement of the incorrect response on the learning of the correct response in the simple T-maze.M. Ray Denny & Morton D. Dunham - 1951 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 41 (5):382.
1 — 50 / 988