Results for 'regular and irregular verbs'

998 found
Order:
  1.  13
    Do morphophonological rules impact both regular and irregular verb inflection? Evidence from acquired morphological impairment.Rimikis Stacey & Buchwald Adam - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  16
    Interpreting dissociations between regular and irregular past-tense morphology.Timothy Justus, Jary Larsen, Paul de Mornay Davies & Diane Swick - 2008 - Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 8 (2):178–194.
    Neuropsychological dissociations between regular and irregular English past-tense morphology have been reported using a lexical decision task in which past-tense primes immediately precede present-tense targets. We present N400 event-related potential data from healthy participants using the same design. Both regular and irregular past-tense forms primed corresponding present-tense forms, but with a longer duration for irregular verbs. Phonological control conditions suggested that differences in formal overlap between prime and target contribute to, but do not account (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  25
    On the processing of regular and irregular forms of verbs and nouns: evidence from neuropsychology.Michele Miozzo - 2003 - Cognition 87 (2):101-127.
  4.  17
    Changes in functional connectivity within the fronto-temporal brain network induced by regular and irregular Russian verb production.Maxim Kireev, Natalia Slioussar, Alexander D. Korotkov, Tatiana V. Chernigovskaya & Svyatoslav V. Medvedev - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  5.  17
    An event-related potential study of cross-modal morphological and phonological priming.Timothy Justus, Jennifer Yang, Jary Larsen, Paul de Mornay Davies & Diane Swick - 2009 - Journal of Neurolinguistics 22 (6):584–604.
    The current work investigated whether differences in phonological overlap between the past- and present-tense forms of regular and irregular verbs can account for the graded neurophysiological effects of verb regularity observed in past-tense priming designs. Event-related potentials were recorded from 16 healthy participants who performed a lexical-decision task in which past-tense primes immediately preceded present-tense targets. To minimize intra-modal phonological priming effects, cross-modal presentation between auditory primes and visual targets was employed, and results were compared to a (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  21
    The role of Broca's area in regular past-tense morphology.Timothy Justus, Jary Larsen, Jennifer Yang, Paul de Mornay Davies, Nina Dronkers & Diane Swick - 2011 - Neuropsychologia 49 (1):1–18.
    It has been suggested that damage to anterior regions of the left hemisphere results in a dissociation in the perception and lexical activation of past-tense forms. Specifically, in a lexical-decision task in which past-tense primes immediately precede present-tense targets, such patients demonstrate significant priming for irregular verbs (spoke–speak), but, unlike control participants, fail to do so for regular verbs (looked–look). Here, this behavioral dissociation was first confirmed in a group of eleven patients with damage to the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  33
    Single mechanism but not single route: Learning verb inflections in constructivist neural networks.Gert Westermann - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1042-1043.
    Clahsen's theory raises problems that make it seem untenable. As an alternative, a constructivist neural network model is reported that develops a modular architecture and in which a single associative mechanism produces all inflections, displaying an emergent dissociation between regular and irregular verbs. Thus, Clahsen's rejection of associative models of inflection concerns only a subgroup of these models.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  42
    Against Regular and Irregular Characterizations of Mechanisms.Lane DesAutels - 2011 - Philosophy of Science 78 (5):914-925.
    This article addresses the question of whether we should conceive of mechanisms as productive of change in a regular way. I argue that, if mechanisms are characterized as fully regular, on the one hand, then not enough processes will count as mechanisms for them to be interesting or useful. If no appeal to regularity is made at all in their characterization, on the other hand, then mechanisms can no longer be useful for grounding prediction and supporting intervention strategies. (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  9.  12
    Overtensing and the effect of regularity.Joseph Paul Stemberger - 2002 - Cognitive Science 26 (6):737-766.
    Regularly inflected forms often behave differently in language production than irregular forms. These differences are often used to argue that irregular forms are listed in the lexicon but regular forms are produced by rule. Using an experimental speech production task with adults, it is shown that overtensing errors, where a tensed verb is used in place of an infinitive, predominantly involve irregular forms, but that the differences may be due to phonological confounds, not to regularity per (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  10.  47
    Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals.Harald Clahsen, Monika Rothweiler, Andreas Woest & Gary F. Marcus - 1992 - Cognition 45 (3):225-255.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  11.  14
    Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals.Harald Clahsen, Monika Rothweiler & Andreas Woest - 1992 - Cognition 45 (3):225-255.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  12. The nature of Regularity and Irregularity: Evidence from Hebrew Nominal Inflection.Steven Pinker & Joseph Shimron - unknown
    Most evidence for the role of regular inflection as a default operation comes from languages that confound the morphological properties of regular and irregular forms with their phonological characteristics. For instance, regular plurals tend to faithfully preserve the base’s phonology, whereas irregular nouns tend to alter it. The distinction between regular and irregular inflection may thus be an epiphenomenon of phonological faithfulness. In Hebrew noun inflection, however, morphological regularity and phonological faithfulness can be (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  13.  51
    Children's Acquisition of the English Past‐Tense: Evidence for a Single‐Route Account From Novel Verb Production Data.Ryan P. Blything, Ben Ambridge & Elena V. M. Lieven - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S2):621-639.
    This study adjudicates between two opposing accounts of morphological productivity, using English past-tense as its test case. The single-route model posits that both regular and irregular past-tense forms are generated by analogy across stored exemplars in associative memory. In contrast, the dual-route model posits that regular inflection requires use of a formal “add -ed” rule that does not require analogy across regular past-tense forms. Children saw animations of an animal performing a novel action described with a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14.  7
    Meaning, Regular and Irregular.J. F. Staal - 1968 - Foundations of Language 4 (2):182-184.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  41
    Dissociation between regular and irregular in connectionist architectures: Two processes, but still no special linguistic rules.Marco Zorzi & Gabriella Vigliocco - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1045-1046.
    Dual-mechanism models of language maintain a distinction between a lexicon and a computational system of linguistic rules. In his target article, Clahsen provides support for such a distinction, presenting evidence from German inflections. He argues for a structured lexicon, going beyond the strict lexicon versus rules dichotomy. We agree with the author in assuming a dual mechanism; however, we argue that a next step must be taken, going beyond the notion of the computational system as specific rules applying to a (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  23
    Transfer following regular and irregular sequences of events in a guessing situation.Lawrence S. Meyers, Erik Driessen & Joseph Halpern - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (2):182.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  17.  42
    Answering the connectionist challenge: a symbolic model of learning the past tenses of English verbs.C. X. Ling & M. Marinov - 1993 - Cognition 49 (3):235-290.
    Supporters of eliminative connectionism have argued for a pattern association-based explanation of language learning and language processing. They deny that explicit rules and symbolic representations play any role in language processing and cognition in general. Their argument is based to a large extent on two artificial neural network (ANN) models that are claimed to be able to learn the past tenses of English verbs (Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986, Parallel distributed processing, Vol. 2, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; MacWhinney & Leinbach, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  18.  7
    A note on the regular and irregular modal systems of Lewis.Bolesław Sobociński - 1962 - Notre Dame Journal of Formal Logic 3 (2):109-113.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  19. Words and rules.Steven Pinker - 1999
    The vast expressive power of language is made possible by two principles: the arbitrary soundmeaning pairing underlying words, and the discrete combinatorial system underlying grammar. These principles implicate distinct cognitive mechanisms: associative memory and symbolmanipulating rules. The distinction may be seen in the difference between regular inflection (e.g., walk-walked), which is productive and open-ended and hence implicates a rule, and irregular inflection (e.g., come-came, which is idiosyncratic and closed and hence implicates individually memorized words. Nonetheless, two very different (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   70 citations  
  20.  25
    Innateness, autonomy, universality, and the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology.David Kemmerer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):639-641.
    Müller's goal of bringing neuroscience to bear on controversies in linguistics is laudable. However, some of his specific proposals about innateness and autonomy are misguided. Recent studies on the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology indicate that these two linguistic processes are subserved by anatomically and physiologically distinct neural subsystems, whose functional organization is likely to be under direct genetic control rather than assembled by strictly epigenetic factors.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  17
    A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.V. Merlin, M. Tataru, F. Valognes, K. Plunkett & P. Juola - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):463-490.
    The acquisition of English noun and verb morphology is modeled using a single-system connectionist network. The network is trained to produce the plurals and past tense forms of a large corpus of monosyllabic English nouns and verbs. The developmental trajectory of network performance is analyzed in detail and is shown to mimic a number of important features of the acquisition of English noun and verb morphology in young children. These include an initial error-free period of performance on both nouns (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  22. The irregular verbs.Steven Pinker - unknown
    The irregulars are defiantly quirky. Thousands of verbs monotonously take the -ed suffix for their past tense forms, but ring mutates to rang, not ringed, catch becomes caught, hit doesn't do anything, and go is replaced by an entirely different word, went (a usurping of the old past tense of to wend, which itself once followed the pattern we see in send-sent and bend-bent). No wonder irregular verbs are banned in "rationally designed" languages like Esperanto and Orwell's (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  23.  61
    Regular versus irregular inflection: A question of levels.Alessandro Laudanna - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1029-1030.
    When referring to the organization of the mental lexicon, the distinction between combinatorial rules and lexical listing for regularly versus irregularly inflected words should be further developed to account for subregular morphological processes. Moreover, the distinction may be more or less appropriate depending on the lexical component under consideration, and it is subject to interplay with other factors that are relevant in determining the representational structure of the lexical system.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  27
    A complex system approach to language evolution.Francesca Colaiori & Francesca Tria - 2020 - Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 2 (2):118-126.
    Regularities in natural language systems, despite their cognitive advantages in terms of storage and learnability, often coexist with exceptions, raising the question of whether and why irregularities survive. We offer a complex system perspective on this issue, focusing on the irregular past tense forms in English. Two separate processes affect the overall regularity: new verbs constantly entering the vocabulary in the regular form at low frequency, and transitions in both directions occurring in a narrow frequency range. The (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  61
    A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.Kim Plunkett & Patrick Juola - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):463-490.
    The acquisition of English noun and verb morphology is modeled using a single-system connectionist network. The network is trained to produce the plurals and past tense forms of a large corpus of monosyllabic English nouns and verbs. The developmental trajectory of network performance is analyzed in detail and is shown to mimic a number of important features of the acquisition of English noun and verb morphology in young children. These include an initial error-free period of performance on both nouns (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  26.  16
    The Search for Regularity in Irregularity: Defectiveness and its Implications for our Knowledge of Words.Marianne Mithun - 2010 - In Mithun Marianne (ed.), Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us. pp. 125.
    The longstanding issue in morphological theory has been the status of inflected forms in the memory. In general, the irregular forms of words are assumed to be learned, stored, and retrieved for use. While the contention on the storage of irregular forms seemed to be clear and cohesive, the views on the nature of storage of regular words vary. For some, all inflected forms are stored while some contend that storage is not homogenous, wherein the frequently-used forms (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  14
    Bolesław Sobociński. A note on the regular and irregular modal systems of Lewis. Notre Dame journal of formal logic, vol. 3 , pp. 109–113. [REVIEW]G. F. Schumm - 1972 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 37 (1):181-182.
  28.  23
    Hogue's Irregular Verbs of Attic Prose The Irregular Verbs of Attic Prose, their forms, prominent meanings, and important compounds; together with lists of related words and English derivatives. By Addison Hogue, Professor of Greek in the University of Mississippi. Ginn and Co., 1889. 6s. [REVIEW]E. C. Marchant - 1890 - The Classical Review 4 (04):166-168.
    Hogue's Irregular Verbs of Attic Prose - The Irregular Verbs of Attic Prose, their forms, prominent meanings, and important compounds; together with lists of related words and English derivatives. By Addison Hogue, Professor of Greek in the University of Mississippi. Ginn and Co., 1889. 6s. - Volume 4 Issue 4.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  12
    On the Verbalization of Space and Direction Concepts.Michail L. Kotin - 2020 - Gestalt Theory 42 (1):5-15.
    Summary The paper deals with selected problems of the verbalization of the concepts “place”, “space” and “direction”, with a special consideration of their successive development in language and in language acquisition. The theoretical background are assumptions concerning the genesis of the concept of place and movement. Some of them claim that movement and direction precede the conceptualization of place and space. However, numerous linguistic phenomena seem to prove the opposite hypothesis, namely that the concept of place and, thus, its verbalization (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  19
    Pronunciation difficulty, temporal regularity, and the speech-to-song illusion.Elizabeth H. Margulis, Rhimmon Simchy-Gross & Justin L. Black - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:122027.
    The speech-to-song illusion ( Deutsch et al., 2011 ) tracks the perceptual transformation from speech to song across repetitions of a brief spoken utterance. Because it involves no change in the stimulus itself, but a dramatic change in its perceived affiliation to speech or to music, it presents a unique opportunity to comparatively investigate the processing of language and music. In this study, native English-speaking participants were presented with brief spoken utterances that were subsequently repeated ten times. The utterances were (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  31.  28
    Another Type of Bilingual Advantage? Tense-Mood-Aspect Frequency, Verb-Form Regularity and Context-Governed Choice in Bilingual vs. Monolingual Spanish Speakers with Agrammatism.O'Connor Wells Barbara & Obler Loraine - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  11
    Regular-, irregular-, and pseudo-character processing in Chinese: The regularity effect in normal adult readers.Lau Dustin Kai Yan, Kong Anthony Pak Hin & Wilson Maximiliano - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  22
    CSR and the workplace attitudes of irregular employees: The case of subcontracted workers in Korea.Mohammad A. Ali & Heung-Jun Jung - 2017 - Business Ethics: A European Review 26 (2):130-146.
    In recent years, there has been a noticeable increase in organizational trends to hire irregular workers. This inclination, in a time of great flux and uncertainty, exacerbates human resource issues faced by firms. We argue that corporate social responsibility can be an important antecedent to improve the workplace attitudes of irregular workers and as a result reduce the negative impact on organizations of the increased use of an irregular workforce. Hence, we explore the relationship between perceived CSR (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  34.  13
    Irregular Negatives, Implicatures, and Idioms.Wayne A. Davis - 2016 - Dordrecht: Springer.
    The author integrates, expands, and deepens his previous publications about irregular (or "metalinguistic") negations. A total of ten distinct negatives-several previously unclassified-are analyzed. The logically irregular negations deny different implicatures of their root. All are partially non-compositional but completely conventional. The author argues that two of the irregular negative meanings are implicatures. The others are semantically rather than pragmatically ambiguous. Since their ambiguity is neither lexical nor structural, direct irregular negatives satisfy the standard definition of idioms (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  35. Descriptions and Tests for Polysemy.Andrei Moldovan - 2021 - Axiomathes 31 (3):229-249.
    Viebahn (2018) has recently argued that several tests for ambiguity, such as the conjunction-reduction test, are not reliable as tests for polysemy, but only as tests for homonymy. I look at the more fine-grained distinction between regular and irregular polysemy and I argue for a more nuanced conclusion: the tests under discussion provide systematic evidence for homonymy and irregular polysemy but need to be used with more care to test for regular polysemy. I put this conclusion (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  36.  33
    The place of analogy in minimalist morphology and the irregularity of regular forms.Dirk P. Janssen - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1025-1026.
    Analogy plays an important role in the production of irregular forms but the proposed Minimalist Morphology (MM) representations do not express this. Recent results also show that the regular forms of strong paradigms can have idiosyncratic properties that cannot be accounted for by MM. Methodological problems with an experiment are discussed and a plea for a processing explanation is made.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  10
    Detecting Evolutionary Forces in Language Change.Mitchell Newberry, Ahern G., A. Christopher, Robin Clark & Joshua B. Plotkin - 2017 - Nature Publishing Group 551 (7679):223–226.
    Both language and genes evolve by transmission over generations with opportunity for differential replication of forms. The understanding that gene frequencies change at random by genetic drift, even in the absence of natural selection, was a seminal advance in evolutionary biology. Stochastic drift must also occur in language as a result of randomness in how linguistic forms are copied between speakers. Here we quantify the strength of selection relative to stochastic drift in language evolution. We use time series derived from (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38. Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: a computational/experimental study.Adam Albright & Bruce Hayes - 2003 - Cognition 90 (2):119-161.
    Are morphological patterns learned in the form of rules? Some models deny this, attributing all morphology to analogical mechanisms. The dual mechanism model (Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1998). On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73-193) posits that speakers do internalize rules, but that these rules are few and cover only regular processes; the remaining patterns are attributed to analogy. This article advocates a third approach, which uses multiple stochastic (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  39.  18
    Illegal, Legal, Irregular or Regular – Who is the Incoming Foreigner?Magdalena Perkowska - 2016 - Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric 45 (1):187-197.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric Jahrgang: 45 Heft: 1 Seiten: 187-197.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  78
    Polysemy: Pragmatics and sense conventions.Robyn Carston - 2021 - Mind and Language 36 (1):108-133.
    Polysemy, understood as instances of a single linguistic expression having multiple related senses, is not a homogenous phenomenon. There are regular (apparently, rule‐based) cases and irregular (resemblance‐based) cases, which have different processing profiles. Although a primary source of polysemy is pragmatic inference, at least some cases become conventionalised and linguistically encoded. Three main issues are discussed: (a) the key differences between regular and irregular cases and the role, if any, of a “core meaning”; (b) the distinction (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  41.  64
    Chance, Experimental Reproducibility, and Mechanistic Regularity.Tudor M. Baetu - 2013 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 27 (3):253-271.
    Examples from the sciences showing that mechanisms do not always succeed in producing the phenomena for which they are responsible have led some authors to conclude that the regularity requirement can be eliminated from characterizations of mechanisms. In this article, I challenge this conclusion and argue that a minimal form of regularity is inextricably embedded in examples of elucidated mechanisms that have been shown to be causally responsible for phenomena. Examples of mechanistic explanations from the sciences involve mechanisms that have (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  42. Dynamical complexity and regularity.Richard Johns - manuscript
    The aim of this paper is to provide a mathematical basis for the plausible idea that regular dynamical laws can only produce (quickly and reliably) regular structures. Thus the actual laws, which are regular, can only produce regular objects, like crystals, and not irregular ones, like living organisms.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  56
    Do Lemmas Speak German? A Verb Position Effect in German Structural Priming.Franklin Chang, Michael Baumann, Sandra Pappert & Hartmut Fitz - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (5):1113-1130.
    Lexicalized theories of syntax often assume that verb-structure regularities are mediated by lemmas, which abstract over variation in verb tense and aspect. German syntax seems to challenge this assumption, because verb position depends on tense and aspect. To examine how German speakers link these elements, a structural priming study was performed which varied syntactic structure, verb position, and verb overlap.structural priming was found, both within and across verb position, but priming was larger when the verb position was the same between (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  44.  43
    The tension between “combinatorial” and “class-default” regularity.Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1017-1018.
    Clahsen shows that “combinatorial” inflection is processed differently from “irregular” inflection. However, combinatorially regular affixes need not coincide with “class-default” affixes, that is, affixes shared by more than one inflection class and all of whose rivals are peculiar to one class. This creates a tension that may help to explain the persistence of inflection class systems.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. Responding to unauthorized residence: on a dilemma between ‘firewalls’ and ‘regularizations’.Lukas Schmid - 2024 - Comparative Migration Studies 12 (22):1-18.
    Residence of unauthorized immigrants is a stable feature of the Global North’s liberal democracies. This article asks how liberal-democratic policymakers should respond to this phenomenon, assuming both that states have incontrovertible rights and interests to assert control over immigration and that unauthorized residence is nevertheless an entrenched fact. It argues that a set of liberal-democratic commitments gives policymakers strong reason to implement both so-called ‘firewall’ and ‘regularization’ policies, thereby protecting unauthorized immigrants’ basic needs and interests and officially incorporating many of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  87
    On the Performance of Indirect Encoding Across the Continuum of Regularity.Kenneth O. Stanley, Robert T. Pennock & Charles Ofria - unknown
    ��This paper investigates how an evolutionary al- gorithm with an indirect encoding exploits the property of phenotypic regularity, an important design principle found in natural organisms and engineered designs. We present the first comprehensive study showing that such phenotypic regularity enables an indirect encoding to outperform direct encoding con- trols as problem regularity increases. Such an ability to produce regular solutions that can exploit the regularity of problems is an important prerequisite if evolutionary algorithms are to scale to high-dimensional (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. Children's Frequency , Productivity Phonology, in the and English Past Tense : The Role of Neighborhood Structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    The productive use of English past tense morphology in school-aged children (N= 74; 3 years, 8 months to 13 years, 5 months) is explored using on elicited production task. Errors represented 20% of the responses overall. Virtually all of the children demonstrated productivity with regular (e.g., good) and irregular patterns (zero-marking, e.g., sit + sit; vowel-change, e.g., ride -+ rid). Overall frequency of errors decreased with age, yet the tendency for certain types of irregularizations increased in the older (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  1
    A Hypothesis on the Genealogy of the Motto “In God We Trust” and the Emergence of the Identity of the Church.Paolo Napoli - 2018 - In Stefan Huygebaert, Angela Condello, Sarah Marusek & Mark Antaki (eds.), Sensing the Nation's Law: Historical Inquiries Into the Aesthetics of Democratic Legitimacy. Cham: Springer Verlag. pp. 191-212.
    The aim of this chapter is to reconstruct a hypothetical genealogy of the U.S. national motto “in God we Trust” by comparing the juridical and theological concept of “depositum”. According to Philo of Alexandria, the deposit was the most sacred institutional act of ancient social life, because it had both a religious and a sociological function. According to the Epistulae to Timothy the term ‘deposit’ defined the legacy of the Christian faith of which the disciple of St. Paul was entrusted. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  11
    Latin Verb Forms.Ernest Riedel - 1916 - Classical Quarterly 10 (03):165-.
    These forms would result regularly from the longer forms, audivisti, etc. The v drops out between two like vowels, and these then contract immediately . Both the long and the short forms are used, but the intervening forms audiisti, etc., do not occur in early Latin, just as diitis is not found.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50. A reexamination of causal irregularity.Steven Lauwers - 1978 - Philosophy of Science 45 (3):471-473.
    Aaron Snyder and Fred Dretske present an argument for the proposition that singular causal sequences, such as S caused b, need not be related to a general regularity, that an event of type S is always followed by an event of type b. Therefore, they assert that a claim of causal relation does not require a regular observation of effect following cause or cause preceeding effect. To reinforce their assertion, they present the following case: Box R contains a randomizing (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 998