Results for 'Stereopsis'

36 found
Order:
  1.  16
    Stereopsis produced without horizontally disparate stimulus loci.Paul C. Squires - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 52 (3):199.
  2.  15
    Stereopsis and binocular rivalry.Jeremy M. Wolfe - 1986 - Psychological Review 93 (3):269-282.
  3.  39
    Machine stereopsis: A feedforward network for fast stereo vision with movable fusion plane.Paul M. Churchland - 1995 - In Android Epistemology. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  4.  12
    For an Epistemology of Stereopsis.Gabriele Ferretti - forthcoming - Review of Philosophy and Psychology:1-18.
    Philosophers and cognitive scientists try to understand, from different perspectives, the nature of the experience of reality. Given this shared, interdisciplinary interest, it would be beneficial to have a coherent story about the experience of reality, in which there is reciprocal contribution from both philosophy and cognitive science. This paper wants to pave the way for this shared enterprise on the investigation of the experience of reality. I first distinguish between two indicators of reality. (1) The experience of availability to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  11
    "Abnormal fusion" of stereopsis and binocular rivalry.Randolph Blake & Robert P. O'Shea - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (1):151-154.
  6. Binocular rivalry and stereopsis revisited.Randolph Blake - 2012 - In Jeremy M. Wolfe & Lynn C. Robertson (eds.), From Perception to Consciousness: Searching with Anne Treisman. Oxford University Press.
  7. The role of stereopsis in prehension.P. Servos & M. Goodale - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (6):481-481.
  8.  12
    Parallel ideas about stereopsis and binocular rivalry: A reply to Blake and O'Shea (1988).Jeremy M. Wolfe - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (1):155-158.
  9.  8
    Psychophysical and computational studies towards a theory of human stereopsis.John E. W. Mayhew & John P. Frisby - 1981 - Artificial Intelligence 17 (1-3):349-385.
  10.  11
    Accuracy of absolute visual distance and size estimation in space as a function of stereopsis and motion parallax.James W. Dees - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (3):466.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  21
    Toward a new theory of stereopsis.Dhanraj Vishwanath - 2014 - Psychological Review 121 (2):151-178.
  12.  13
    Perception of slant when perspective and stereopsis conflict: Experiments with aniseikonic lenses.B. J. Gillam - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 78 (2p1):299.
  13.  17
    A theoretical analysis of illusory contour formation in stereopsis.Barton L. Anderson & Bela Julesz - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (4):705-743.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  14.  19
    On the Relationship Between Sensory Eye Dominance and Stereopsis in the Normal-Sighted Adult Population: Normative Data.Yonghua Wang, Lele Cui, Zhifen He, Wenman Lin, Jia Qu, Fan Lu, Jiawei Zhou & Robert F. Hess - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  15. Seeing in three dimensions: the neurophysiology of stereopsis.C. Gregory & L. DeAngelis - 2000 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4:3.
  16. Early vision detects differential structure prior to stereopsis.J. S. Lappin & W. D. Craft - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (6):488-488.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17. The influence of eye movements on stereopsis.V. Lyakhovetskii & E. P. Popechitelev - 2004 - In Robert Schwartz (ed.), Perception. Malden Ma: Blackwell. pp. 98-98.
  18.  24
    Toward a new theory of stereopsis: A critique of Vishwanath (2014).Brian Rogers - 2019 - Psychological Review 126 (1):162-169.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19. Localisation and identification of illusory surface with binocular stereopsis.D. Yoshino & M. Idesawa - 2004 - In Robert Schwartz (ed.), Perception. Malden Ma: Blackwell. pp. 81-81.
  20.  14
    Toward a general theory of stereopsis: Binocular matching, occluding contours, and fusion.Barton L. Anderson & Ken Nakayama - 1994 - Psychological Review 101 (3):414-445.
  21.  4
    Advancing a new theory of stereopsis: Reply to Rogers (2019).Dhanraj Vishwanath - 2020 - Psychological Review 127 (1):146-152.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  22
    Levels of modeling of mechanisms of visually guided behavior.Michael A. Arbib - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):407-436.
    Intermediate constructs are required as bridges between complex behaviors and realistic models of neural circuitry. For cognitive scientists in general, schemas are the appropriate functional units; brain theorists can work with neural layers as units intermediate between structures subserving schemas and small neural circuits.After an account of different levels of analysis, we describe visuomotor coordination in terms of perceptual schemas and motor schemas. The interest of schemas to cognitive science in general is illustrated with the example of perceptual schemas in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  23.  12
    Effects of matrix elements on steropsis and anomalous contour.R. B. Lawson & R. J. Pandina - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (2):322.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24. Seeing, visualizing, and believing: Pictures and cognitive penetration.John Zeimbekis - 2015 - In John Zeimbekis & Athanassios Raftopoulos (eds.), The Cognitive Penetrability of Perception: New Philosophical Perspectives. Oxford University Press. pp. 298-327.
    Visualizing and mental imagery are thought to be cognitive states by all sides of the imagery debate. Yet the phenomenology of those states has distinctly visual ingredients. This has potential consequences for the hypothesis that vision is cognitively impenetrable, the ability of visual processes to ground perceptual warrant and justification, and the distinction between cognitive and perceptual phenomenology. I explore those consequences by describing two forms of visual ambiguity that involve visualizing: the ability to visually experience a picture surface as (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  25.  99
    Individuating Cognitive Characters: Lessons from Praying Mantises and Plants.Carrie Figdor - forthcoming - Philosophy of Science.
    This paper advances the development of a phylogeny-based psychology in which cognitive ability types are individuated as characters in the evolutionary biological sense. I explain the character concept and its utility in addressing (or dissolving) conceptual problems arising from discoveries of cognitive abilities across a wide range of species. I use the examples of stereopsis in the praying mantis, internal cell-to-cell signaling in plants, and episodic memory in scrub jays to show how anthropocentric cognitive ability types can be reformulated (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  62
    Perception and action in depth.D. P. Carey, H. Chris Dijkerman & A. David Milner - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):438-453.
    Little is known about distance processing in patients with posterior brain damage. Although many investigators have claimed that distance estimates are normal or abnormal in some of these patients, many of these observations were made informally and the examiners often asked for relative, and not absolute, distance estimates. The present investigation served two purposes. First, we wanted to contrast the use of distance information in peripersonal space for perceptual report as opposed to visuomotor control in our visual form agnosic patient, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  27. Quantifying the Relative Roles of Shadows, Steropsis, and Aocal Accomodation in 3D Visualization.David Kirsh - 2003 - The 3rd IASTED International Conference on Visualization, Imaging, and Image Processing.
    The goal of three-dimensional visualization is to present information in such a way that the viewer suspends disbelief and uses the screen imagery the same way as he or she would use an identical, real 3D scene. To do this effectively, programmers employ a variety of 3D depth cues. Our own anecdotal experience says that shadows and stereopsis are two of the best for visualization. The nice thing is that both of these are possible to do in interactive programs. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  7
    Metagnosis: Revelatory Narratives of Health and Identity by Danielle Spencer.Élaina Gauthier-Mamaril - 2022 - International Journal of Feminist Approaches to Bioethics 15 (1):198-202.
    Metagnosis, as a text, is an exercise in metanarration: Throughout the book, Danielle Spencer pulls together medical and medicalized storytelling and self-identification accounts to make sense of a plot device that had remained unnamed. "Metagnosis," as coined by Spencer, refers to the dynamic process of learning later in life that one has a medical condition or that part of oneself can now be medicalized. For example, Spencer recounts how "discovering" her lack of stereopsis as an adult affected her understanding (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  83
    Study of Geometric Illusory Visual Perception – A New Perspective in the Functional Evaluation of Children With Strabismus.Juliana Tessari Dias Rohr, Cassiano Rodrigues Isaac, Adriano de Almeida de Lima, Ana Garcia, Procópio Miguel dos Santos & Maria Clotilde Henriques Tavares - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Despite the various perceptual-motor deficits documented in strabismus, there is a paucity of studies evaluating visual illusions in patients with strabismus. The aim of this study was to examine how the illusionary perception occurs in children/adolescents with strabismus with referral for surgery to correct ocular deviations. A controlled cross-sectional study was carried out in which 45 participants with strabismus and 62 healthy volunteers aged 10–15 years were evaluated. The behavioral response to three geometric illusions [Vertical-Horizontal illusion, Müller-Lyer illusion and Ponzo (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  32
    Mental functions as constraints on neurophysiology: Biology and psychology of vision.Gary Hatfield - 1999 - In V. Harcastle (ed.), Where Biology Meets Psychology. pp. 251--71.
    This chapter examines a question at the intersection of the mind-body problem and the analysis of mental representation: the question of the direction of constraint between psychological fact and theory and neurophysiological or physical fact and theory. Does physiology constrain psychology? Are physiological facts more basic than psychological facts? Or do psychological theories, including representational analyses, guide and constrain physiology? Despite the antireductionist bent of functionalist positions, it has generally been assumed that physics or physiology are more basic than, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  31. Seeing and summing: Implications of computational theories of vision.Austen Clark - 1984 - Cognition and Brain Theory 7 (1):1-23.
    Marr's computational theory of stereopsis is shown to imply that human vision employs a system of representation which has all the properties of a number system. Claims for an internal number system and for neural computation should be taken literally. I show how these ideas withstand various skeptical attacks, and analyze the requirements for describing neural operations as computations. Neural encoding of numerals is shown to be distinct from our ability to measure visual physiology. The constructs in Marr's theory (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  17
    Vector code differences and similarities.E. N. Sokolov - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (4):479-480.
    Edelman suggests that any shape is encoded by an excitation vector with components corresponding to excitations of corresponding neuronal modules. This results in discrimination of stimuli in a shape space of low dimensionality. Similar vector encoding is present in color vision. Red-green, blue-yellow, bright and dark neurons are modules that represent a number of different color stimuli in color space of low dimensionality. Vector encoding allows effective computation of color differences and color similarities. Such a neuronal vector-encoding approach has also (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33. Common functional pathways for texture and form vision: A single case study.Lucia M. Vaina - 1990 - Synthese 83 (1):93-131.
    A single case study of a patient, D.M., with a lesion in the region of the right occipito-temporal gyrus is presented. D.M. had well-preserved language and general cognitive abilities. Colour discrimination, contrast sensitivity, gross depth perception, spatial localization, and motion appreciation were within normal limits.On the evaluation of perceptual abilities, he failed to identify two-dimensional shapes from stereoscopic vision, motion, and texture although in all cases he was able to identify the rough area subtended by the shape. These findings are (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  30
    Segregation and integration of information among visual modules.Giorgio Vallortigara - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (3):398-399.
    It is argued that the alleged cases of cognitive penetration of visual modules actually arise from the integration of information among different modules. This would reflect a general computational strategy according to which constraints to a particular module would be provided by information coming from different modules. Examples are provided from the integration of stereopsis and occlusion and from computation of motion direction.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35. What Can the Mind Tell Us About the Brain? Psychology, Neurophysiology, and Constraint.Gary Hatfield - 2009 - In Perception and Cognition: Essays in the Philosophy of Psychology. Clarendon Press. pp. 434-55.
    This chapter examines the relations between psychology and neuroscience. There is a strong philosophical intuition that direct study of the brain can and will constrain the development of psychological theory. When this intuition is tested against case studies from the psychology of perception and memory, it turns out that psychology has led the way toward knowledge of neurophysiology. The chapter presents an abstract argument to show that psychology can and must lead the way in neuroscientific study of mental function. The (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  36. The brain's 'new' science: Psychology, neurophysiology, and constraint.Gary Hatfield - 2000 - Philosophy of Science 67 (3):388-404.
    There is a strong philosophical intuition that direct study of the brain can and will constrain the development of psychological theory. When this intuition is tested against case studies on the neurophysiology and psychology of perception and memory, it turns out that psychology has led the way toward knowledge of neurophysiology. An abstract argument is developed to show that psychology can and must lead the way in neuroscientific study of mental function. The opposing intuition is based on mainly weak arguments (...)
    Direct download (16 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations