Results for ' word pairs'

991 found
Order:
  1.  6
    Effects of Sleep on Word Pair Memory in Children – Separating Item and Source Memory Aspects.Jing-Yi Wang, Frederik D. Weber, Katharina Zinke, Hannes Noack & Jan Born - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  2.  27
    Processing negative valence of word pairs that include a positive word.Oksana Itkes & Nira Mashal - 2016 - Cognition and Emotion 30 (6).
  3.  34
    Cross‐Situational Learning of Minimal Word Pairs.Paola Escudero, Karen E. Mulak & Haley A. Vlach - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (2):455-465.
    Cross-situational statistical learning of words involves tracking co-occurrences of auditory words and objects across time to infer word-referent mappings. Previous research has demonstrated that learners can infer referents across sets of very phonologically distinct words, but it remains unknown whether learners can encode fine phonological differences during cross-situational statistical learning. This study examined learners’ cross-situational statistical learning of minimal pairs that differed on one consonant segment, minimal pairs that differed on one vowel segment, and non-minimal pairs (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  4.  9
    Stylistic Studies of Word-Pairs in Biblical and Ancient Semitic Literatures.David Marcus & Yitzhak Avishur - 1989 - Journal of the American Oriental Society 109 (4):684.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  44
    Formation of semantic associations between subliminally presented face-word pairs.Simone B. Duss, Sereina Oggier, Thomas P. Reber & Katharina Henke - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (3):928-935.
    Recent evidence suggests that consciousness of encoding is not necessary for the rapid formation of new semantic associations. We investigated whether unconsciously formed associations are as semantically precise as would be expected for associations formed with consciousness of encoding during episodic memory formation. Pairs of faces and written occupations were presented subliminally for unconscious associative encoding. Five minutes later, the same faces were presented suprathreshold for the cued unconscious retrieval of face-occupation associations. Retrieval instructions required participants to classify the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  6.  46
    Subliminal understanding of negation: Unconscious control by subliminal processing of word pairs.Anna-Marie Armstrong & Zoltan Dienes - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (3):1022-1040.
    A series of five experiments investigated the extent of subliminal processing of negation. Participants were presented with a subliminal instruction to either pick or not pick an accompanying noun, followed by a choice of two nouns. By employing subjective measures to determine individual thresholds of subliminal priming, the results of these studies indicated that participants were able to identify the correct noun of the pair – even when the correct noun was specified by negation. Furthermore, using a grey-scale contrast method (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  7.  1
    Encoding specificity and integration of word pairs.Charles De Vito - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 5 (3):215-216.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  17
    Cued recall performance of unrelated word pairs is reduced by the presence of A-B, A-C items in the same list.Donald Robbins, James F. Bray & James R. Irvin - 1977 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 9 (2):121-123.
  9.  15
    Commentary: Effects of Sleep on Word Pair Memory in Children–Separating Item and Source Memory Aspects.Jennifer M. Johnson & Simon J. Durrant - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations.David E. Meyer & Roger W. Schvaneveldt - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 90 (2):227.
  11.  24
    Fast Pairs: A visual word recognition paradigm for measuring entrenchment, top-down effects, and subjective phenomenology☆.Catherine L. Caldwell-Harris & Alison L. Morris - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (4):1063-1081.
    When word pairs having a familiar order are sequentially flashed on a computer in their non-familiar order, , observers have a strong phenomenology of seeing them in familiar order . Reversal errors remained frequent even when participants obtained perceptual experience of reverse-display items by beginning with a block of longer-duration trials. A forced-choice order-detection procedure reduced but did not eliminate reversal errors, showing that “fast pairs” is a robust perceptual illusion. Even adjective + noun pairs showed (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  12.  26
    Effects of two- word stimuli on recall and learning in a paired-associate task.Barabara S. Musgrave & Jean Carl Cohen - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (2):161.
  13.  20
    Effects of word order and imagery on learning verbs and adverbs as paired associates.James L. Pate, Patricia Ward & Katherine B. Harlan - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (4):792.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  23
    The differential effects of word and object stimuli on the learning of paired associates.C. C. Wimer & W. E. Lambert - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 57 (1):31.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  26
    Stimulus selection in paired-associate learning: Consonant-triad versus word-triad paradigms.Franklin M. Berry & Steven R. Cole - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 97 (3):402.
  16.  23
    Effects of word frequency and acoustic similarity on free-recall and paired-associate-recognition learning.Stephen W. Holborn, Karen L. Gross & Pamela A. Catlin - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 101 (1):169.
  17.  20
    Transition probability, word order, and noun abstractness in the learning of adjective-noun paired associates.Igor Kusyszyn & Allan Paivio - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (6):800.
  18.  23
    Tachistoscopic recognition thresholds, paired-associate learning, and free recall as a function of abstractness-concreteness and word frequency.Wilma A. Winnick & Kenneth Kressel - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (2):163.
  19.  20
    On the Interweaving of Words with Pairs of Parallel Phrases.R. S. Conway - 1900 - The Classical Review 14 (07):357-360.
  20.  11
    Cued recall for four-word categories presented in separate pairs.George A. Weigel, Joel D. Schendel & Henry M. Halff - 1980 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 15 (5):361-364.
  21.  19
    The selective perception and recognition of single words from competing dichotic stimulus pairs.G. Bonanno - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (3):241-264.
    Five experiments are reported that concern selective perception and representation following dichotic presentations of competing word pairs differing only in their initial consonants . Only one word from each pair tended to be subjectively perceived, even when participants were encouraged to guess two words. Robust selective perception effects were evidenced as a function of stimulus affective valence. Control tasks showed that these effects could not be attributed to report biases or to the acoustic properties of the stimuli. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  14
    Same and different judgments for word-color pairs with "irrelevant" words or colors: Evidence for word-code comparisons.Frederick N. Dyer - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (1):102.
  23.  17
    Stimulus frequency and meaningfulness varied independently in the learning of word-number pairs.Marian Schwartz - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 87 (2):289.
  24.  10
    Memory for pleasant, unpleasant, and indifferent pairs of words.H. Cason & F. C. Lungren - 1932 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 15 (6):728.
  25.  6
    Event-frequency judgments as a function of the linguistic frequency and single or paired presentation of target words: I. Task with unique multiple traces.Melvin H. Marx - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (4):245-247.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26.  12
    Event-frequency judgments as a function of the linguistic frequency and single or paired presentation of target words: II Task requiring judgment of linguistic frequency.Melvin H. Marx - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (5):361-364.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27.  53
    Hard Words.Lila R. Gleitman, Anna Papafragou & John C. Trueswell - unknown
    How do children acquire the meaning of words? And why are words such as know harder for learners to acquire than words such as dog or jump? We suggest that the chief limiting factor in acquiring the vocabulary of natural languages consists not in overcoming conceptual difficulties with abstract word meanings but rather in mapping these meanings onto their corresponding lexical forms. This opening premise of our position, while controversial, is shared with some prior approaches. The present discussion moves (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  28.  32
    Words Get in the Way: Linguistic Effects on Talker Discrimination.Chandan R. Narayan, Lorinda Mak & Ellen Bialystok - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (5):1361-1376.
    A speech perception experiment provides evidence that the linguistic relationship between words affects the discrimination of their talkers. Listeners discriminated two talkers' voices with various linguistic relationships between their spoken words. Listeners were asked whether two words were spoken by the same person or not. Word pairs varied with respect to the linguistic relationship between the component words, forming either: phonological rhymes, lexical compounds, reversed compounds, or unrelated pairs. The degree of linguistic relationship between the words affected (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  29.  31
    Easy Words: Reference Resolution in a Malevolent Referent World.Lila R. Gleitman & John C. Trueswell - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):22-47.
    Gleitman and Trueswell’s “The easy words” forms a pair with their earlier paper, “Hard words,” completing a circle in which the authors ask how “easy” words (e.g., concrete nouns) are learned. They take up the hypothesis of “cross‐situational learning,” and argue that accumulating observations actually hinders learning if the mechanism requires holding all exemplars in memory over time. They present an alternative hypothesis, “Propose but Verify,” wherein people use one‐trial learning to confirm or disconfirm their current hypothesis—a mechanism distinctly different (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  30. Words and rules.Steven Pinker - 1999
    The vast expressive power of language is made possible by two principles: the arbitrary soundmeaning pairing underlying words, and the discrete combinatorial system underlying grammar. These principles implicate distinct cognitive mechanisms: associative memory and symbolmanipulating rules. The distinction may be seen in the difference between regular inflection (e.g., walk-walked), which is productive and open-ended and hence implicates a rule, and irregular inflection (e.g., come-came, which is idiosyncratic and closed and hence implicates individually memorized words. Nonetheless, two very different theories have (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   70 citations  
  31.  29
    Learning words from sights and sounds: a computational model.Deb K. Roy & Alex P. Pentland - 2002 - Cognitive Science 26 (1):113-146.
    This paper presents an implemented computational model of word acquisition which learns directly from raw multimodal sensory input. Set in an information theoretic framework, the model acquires a lexicon by finding and statistically modeling consistent cross‐modal structure. The model has been implemented in a system using novel speech processing, computer vision, and machine learning algorithms. In evaluations the model successfully performed speech segmentation, word discovery and visual categorization from spontaneous infant‐directed speech paired with video images of single objects. (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  32.  4
    Color-word value index.Anne Thompson - 2013 - Rosendale, NY: Women's Studio Workshop.
    A reference volume and playful devotional object based on the author's research into color theory informed by psychology, mysticism, and early modernism. The Color-Word Value Index has 44 word pairings; 10 colors; 5 suits; and values of positive, negative, and neutral"--Women's Studio Workshop website, viewed December 14, 2021.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33. Words Fail Me. (Stanley Cavell's Life out of Music).William Day - 2020 - In David LaRocca (ed.), Inheriting Stanley Cavell: Memories, Dreams, Reflections. New York: Bloomsbury. pp. 187-97.
    Stanley Cavell isn't the first to arrive at philosophy through a life with music. Nor is he the first whose philosophical practice bears the marks of that life. Much of Cavell's life with music is confirmed for the world in his philosophical autobiography Little Did I Know. A central moment in that book is Cavell's describing the realization that he was to leave his musical career behind – for what exactly, he did not yet know. He connects the memory-shock of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  34.  42
    Iconic Gestures Prime Words.De-Fu Yap, Wing-Chee So, Ju-Min Melvin Yap, Ying-Quan Tan & Ruo-Li Serene Teoh - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (1):171-183.
    Using a cross‐modal semantic priming paradigm, both experiments of the present study investigated the link between the mental representations of iconic gestures and words. Two groups of the participants performed a primed lexical decision task where they had to discriminate between visually presented words and nonwords (e.g., flirp). Word targets (e.g., bird) were preceded by video clips depicting either semantically related (e.g., pair of hands flapping) or semantically unrelated (e.g., drawing a square with both hands) gestures. The duration of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  35. New Words for Old Wrongs.Ishani Maitra - 2018 - Episteme 15 (3):345-362.
    This paper begins with the idea that there are sometimes gaps in our shared linguistic/ conceptual resources that make it difficult for us to understand our own social experiences, and to make them intelligible to others. In this paper, I focus on three cases of this sort, some of which are drawn from the literature on hermeneutical injustice. I offer a diagnosis of what the gaps in these cases consist in, and what it takes to fill them. I argue that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  36.  50
    Iconic Gestures Prime Words.De-Fu Yap, Wing-Chee So, Ju-Min Melvin Yap, Ying-Quan Tan & Ruo-Li Serene Teoh - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (1):171-183.
    Using a cross-modal semantic priming paradigm, both experiments of the present study investigated the link between the mental representations of iconic gestures and words. Two groups of the participants performed a primed lexical decision task where they had to discriminate between visually presented words and nonwords (e.g., flirp). Word targets (e.g., bird) were preceded by video clips depicting either semantically related (e.g., pair of hands flapping) or semantically unrelated (e.g., drawing a square with both hands) gestures. The duration of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  37.  17
    Mediated association in the paired-associate learning of children using heterogeneous and homogeneous lists.David S. Palermo - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (5):711.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  16
    Context factors in paired-associate learning and recall.Donald M. Sundland & Delos D. Wickens - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (3):302.
  39.  4
    An ERP Study on the Role of Phonological Processing in Reading Two-Character Compound Chinese Words of High and Low Frequency.Yuling Wang, Minghu Jiang, Yunlong Huang & Peijun Qiu - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Unlike in English, the role of phonology in word recognition in Chinese is unclear. In this event-related potential experiment, we investigated the role of phonology in reading both high- and low-frequency two-character compound Chinese words. Participants executed semantic and homophone judgment tasks of the same precede-target pairs. Each pair of either high- or low-frequency words were either unrelated or related semantically or phonologically. The induced P200 component was greater for low- than for high-frequency word-pairs both in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  40.  60
    A Probabilistic Computational Model of Cross-Situational Word Learning.Afsaneh Fazly, Afra Alishahi & Suzanne Stevenson - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (6):1017-1063.
    Words are the essence of communication: They are the building blocks of any language. Learning the meaning of words is thus one of the most important aspects of language acquisition: Children must first learn words before they can combine them into complex utterances. Many theories have been developed to explain the impressive efficiency of young children in acquiring the vocabulary of their language, as well as the developmental patterns observed in the course of lexical acquisition. A major source of disagreement (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  41.  13
    Word made skin: figuring language at the surface of flesh.Karmen MacKendrick - 2004 - New York: Fordham University Press.
    Today, body and language are prominent themes throughout philosophy. Each is strange enough on its own; this book asks what sense we might make of them together. Words reach out. Hands pick up books; eyes or fingertips scan text. But just where, if at all, do words and bodies touch? In a trio of paired chapters, each juxtaposing an illustrative story or case study to a theoretical exploration, MacKendrick examines three somatic figures of speech: the touch, the fold, and the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  15
    Compound stimuli in paired-associate learning.Leonard M. Horowitz, Louis G. Kippman & George W. McConkie - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (2):132.
  43.  10
    Detection of words versus good old counting: A note on Mizrahi and Dickinson, “The analytic‐continental divide in philosophical practice”.Hugo Dirk Hogenbirk - 2023 - Metaphilosophy 54 (5):734-745.
    In a recent Metaphilosophy article, Moti Mizrahi and Michael Dickinson argue against characterizing the divide between analytical and continental philosophy as a divide in the use of arguments. This hypothesis is rejected on the basis of a text‐mining approach. The present paper argues that the results they extracted do not answer the questions they set out to answer as well as would have been possible. This is due to Mizrahi and Dickinson's choice to disregard duplicate occurrences of argument word (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  8
    A Co-Word Analysis of Global Research on Knowledge Organization: 1900-2019.Ali Akbar Khasseh, Faramarz Soheili & Omid Alipour - 2022 - Knowledge Organization 49 (5):303-315.
    The study’s objective is to analyze the structure of knowledge organization studies conducted worldwide. This applied research has been conducted with a scientometrics approach using the co-word analysis. The research records consisted of all articles published in the journals of Knowledge Organization and Cataloging & Classification Quarterly and keywords related to the field of knowledge organization indexed in Web of Science from 1900 to 2019, in which 17,950 records were analyzed entirely with plain text format. The total number of (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  18
    Words as sets of features: Processing phonological cues.Douglas L. Nelson, David H. Brooks & John R. Fosselman - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (3):305.
  46.  14
    What Color Is Your Anger? Assessing Color-Emotion Pairings in English Speakers.Jennifer Marie Binzak Fugate & Courtny L. Franco - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Do English-speakers think about anger as “red” and sadness as “blue”? Some theories of emotion suggests that color(s) - like other biologically-derived signals- should be reliably paired with an emotion, and that colors should differentiate across emotions. We assessed consistency and specificity for color-emotion pairings among English-speaking adults. In study 1, participants (n = 73) completed an online survey in which they could select up to three colors from 23 colored swatches (varying hue, saturation, and light) for each of ten (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47.  12
    Conditions that determine effectiveness of picture-mediated paired-associate learning.Keith A. Wollen & Douglas H. Lowry - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (1):181.
  48.  25
    Effects that a massed repetition of one pair has on other pairs in a list.George W. McConkie - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (1p1):187.
  49. Exclusion Constraints Facilitate Statistical Word Learning.Katherine Yoshida, Mijke Rhemtulla & Athena Vouloumanos - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (5):933-947.
    The roles of linguistic, cognitive, and social-pragmatic processes in word learning are well established. If statistical mechanisms also contribute to word learning, they must interact with these processes; however, there exists little evidence for such mechanistic synergy. Adults use co-occurrence statistics to encode speech–object pairings with detailed sensitivity in stochastic learning environments (Vouloumanos, 2008). Here, we replicate this statistical work with nonspeech sounds and compare the results with the previous speech studies to examine whether exclusion constraints contribute equally (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50. “Bamboozled by Our Own Words”: Semantic Blindness and Some Arguments Against Contextualism.Keith Derose - 2006 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 73 (2):316 - 338.
    The best grounds for accepting contextualism concerning knowledge attributions are to be found in how knowledge-attributing (and knowledge-denying) sentences are used in ordinary, nonphilosophical talk: What ordinary speakers will count as “knowledge” in some non-philosophical contexts they will deny is such in others. Contextualists typically appeal to pairs of cases that forcefully display the variability in the epistemic standards that govern ordinary usage: A “low standards” case (henceforth, “LOW”) in which a speaker seems quite appropriately and truthfully to ascribe (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
1 — 50 / 991