Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Opponent-process theory: The interaction of trials, intertrial interval, and the presence of evoking stimuli.Donald R. Yelen - 1985 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23 (1):25-27.
  • Avoidance is in the head, not the genes.Everett J. Wyers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-685.
  • A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction.Roy A. Wise & Michael A. Bozarth - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):469-492.
  • Conditioned immune responses: How are they mediated and how are they related to other classically conditioned responses?Jay M. Weiss - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):412-413.
  • Determinants of Emotion Duration and Underlying Psychological and Neural Mechanisms.Philippe Verduyn, Pauline Delaveau, Jean-Yves Rotgé, Philippe Fossati & Iven Van Mechelen - 2015 - Emotion Review 7 (4):330-335.
    Emotions are traditionally considered to be brief states that last for seconds or a few minutes at most. However, due to pioneering theoretical work of Frijda and recent empirical studies, it has become clear that the duration of emotions is actually highly variable with durations ranging from a few seconds to several hours, or even longer. We review research on determinants of emotion duration. Three classes of determinants are identified: features related to the emotion-eliciting event, emotion itself, and emotion-experiencing person. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Is conditioned immunosuppression an adequate research strategy?H. Dick Veldhuis & David De Wied - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):411-412.
  • The interpersonal theory of suicide.Kimberly A. Van Orden, Tracy K. Witte, Kelly C. Cukrowicz, Scott R. Braithwaite, Edward A. Selby & Thomas E. Joiner - 2010 - Psychological Review 117 (2):575-600.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Pain is pain and fear is fear.Holger Ursin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):318-320.
  • Advances toward a biological theory of aversive learning: Flirtation or commitment?Dallas Treit & Marcia L. Spetch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):684-685.
  • Action representations in prevention behavior: Evidence from motor execution.Solveig Tonn, Moritz Schaaf, Wilfried Kunde & Roland Pfister - 2023 - Cognition 234 (C):105370.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Frontal Brain Asymmetry and Depression: A Self-regulatory Perspective.Andrew J. Tomarkenand & Anita D. Keener - 1998 - Cognition and Emotion 12 (3):387-420.
    Recent findings indicate that frontal brain asymmetry may be a marker of for depression. However, the psychological predispositions that account linkage between frontal brain asymmetry and depression are unclear. approach-withdrawal hypothesis is the primary framework that has been to account for the linkages between frontal brain asymmetry and or emotional disorders. We review evidence consistent with this and suggest several directions for its extension. One such direction is to constrain the approach-withdrawal hypothesis by linking frontal asymmetry to the known functions (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • A cognitive-incentive view.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):683-684.
  • The emerging field of psychoneuroimmunology.George Freeman Solomon - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):411-411.
  • Effects of word repetition and presentation rate on the frequency of verbal transformations: Support for habituation.Katharine A. Snyder, Richard S. Calef, Michael C. Choban & E. Scott Geller - 1993 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31 (2):91-93.
  • Pituitary-adrenal system involvement in conditioned immune changes: Perhaps suppressions are playing a role.William P. Smotherman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):410-410.
  • Psychoneuroimmunology, psychopharmacology, and synthetic physiology.Shepard Siegel & Michael T. Scoles - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):409-410.
  • Naloxone antagonism of hyperactivity in morphine-treated hamsters.Paul Schnur, David Hang & Audra Stinchcomb - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (6):482-485.
  • Counter-regulation triggered by emotions: Positive/negative affective states elicit opposite valence biases in affective processing.Susanne Schwager & Klaus Rothermund - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (5):839-855.
  • Conditioned opponent responses in human tolerance to caffeine.Paul Rozin, Donna Reff, Michael Mark & Jonathan Schull - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (2):117-120.
  • The condition of immunology.Leon T. Rosenberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):408-408.
  • Behavioral conditioning of immunomodulation.Thomas Roszman - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):408-409.
  • The meaning of learning.Anthony L. Riley - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):407-408.
  • Questions about conditioned immunosuppression and biological adaptation.Sam Revusky - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):407-407.
  • A unified framework for addiction: Vulnerabilities in the decision process.A. David Redish, Steve Jensen & Adam Johnson - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (4):415-437.
    The understanding of decision-making systems has come together in recent years to form a unified theory of decision-making in the mammalian brain as arising from multiple, interacting systems (a planning system, a habit system, and a situation-recognition system). This unified decision-making system has multiple potential access points through which it can be driven to make maladaptive choices, particularly choices that entail seeking of certain drugs or behaviors. We identify 10 key vulnerabilities in the system: (1) moving away from homeostasis, (2) (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Toward a general theory of infantile attachment: a comparative review of aspects of the social bond.D. W. Rajecki, Michael E. Lamb & Pauline Obmascher - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (3):417-436.
  • The impact of minimal exposure to affective information on mood and its moderation by prime visibility: a meta-analysis.Nicolas Pillaud & François Ric - 2023 - Cognition and Emotion 37 (2):182-195.
    This article presents a meta-analysis of the impact of minimal exposure to affective stimuli on the emergence of enduring conscious affective feelings. Theories often assume that such affective feelings are linked to automatic appraisals of events (i.e. in the absence of an evaluative processing goal). However, few studies have tested this hypothesis. Moreover, they have provided divergent results. We propose a meta-analysis of these studies to get a clearer picture on this issue. The meta-analysis includes 22 studies (37 effect sizes; (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Given the chance, the normal brain can casually avoid what it would otherwise intensely fear.Jaak Panksepp & Larry Normansell - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):682-683.
  • Reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Arlo K. Myers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):681-682.
  • The Neural Correlates of Cued Reward Omission.Jessica A. Mollick, Luke J. Chang, Anjali Krishnan, Thomas E. Hazy, Kai A. Krueger, Guido K. W. Frank, Tor D. Wager & Randall C. O’Reilly - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Compared to our understanding of positive prediction error signals occurring due to unexpected reward outcomes, less is known about the neural circuitry in humans that drives negative prediction errors during omission of expected rewards. While classical learning theories such as Rescorla–Wagner or temporal difference learning suggest that both types of prediction errors result from a simple subtraction, there has been recent evidence suggesting that different brain regions provide input to dopamine neurons which contributes to specific components of this prediction error (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Distancing-Embracing model of the enjoyment of negative emotions in art reception.Winfried Menninghaus, Valentin Wagner, Julian Hanich, Eugen Wassiliwizky, Thomas Jacobsen & Stefan Koelsch - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40:e347.
    Why are negative emotions so central in art reception far beyond tragedy? Revisiting classical aesthetics in the light of recent psychological research, we present a novel model to explain this much discussed (apparent) paradox. We argue that negative emotions are an important resource for the arts in general, rather than a special license for exceptional art forms only. The underlying rationale is that negative emotions have been shown to be particularly powerful in securing attention, intense emotional involvement, and high memorability, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Progress toward a general theory of health.Theodore Melnechuk - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):406-407.
  • Avoidance behavior: Assumptions, theory, and metatheory.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-696.
  • Conditioning as compensation?N. J. Mackintosh - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):318-318.
  • Expecting shock.A. W. Logue - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):680-681.
  • Inside and Outside: The Relation between Emotional States and Expressions.Michael Lewis - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (2):189-196.
    The association between emotional expression and physiological emotional states is at best, modest. Using data from the autonomic nervous system (ANS), central nervous system (CNS), and hormonal systems there appears to be an association which accounts for approximately 10—20% of the variance between them. Excluding measurement error, it is proposed that the need for action and regulation accounts for the low levels of synchrony. Understanding system responses allows for the study of individual differences as a way of understanding both emotional (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Relationship of Emotion to Cognition: A Functional Approach to a Semantic Controversy.Howard Leventhal & Klaus Scherer - 1987 - Cognition and Emotion 1 (1):3-28.
  • Multiattribute Decision Making in Context: A Dynamic Neural Network Methodology.Samuel J. Leven & Daniel S. Levine - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (2):271-299.
    A theoretical structure for multiattribute decision making is presented, based on a dynamical system for interactions in a neural network incorporating affective and rational variables. This enables modeling of problems that elude two prevailing economic decision theories: subjective expected utility theory and prospect theory. The network is unlike some that fit economic data by choosing optimal weights or coefficients within a predetermined mathematical framework. Rather, the framework itself is based on principles used elsewhere to model many other cognitive and behavioral (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Benefits of Pain.Siri Leknes & Brock Bastian - 2014 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 5 (1):57-70.
    Pain is most often an unpleasant experience that alerts us to actual or possible tissue damage. However, insisting that pain is always bad news may hinder understanding of pain’s many facets. Despite its unpleasantness – or perhaps because of it – pain is known to enhance the perceived value of certain activities, such as punishment or endurance sports. Here, we review evidence for a series of mechanisms involved in putative benefits of pain. A byproduct of pain’s attention-grabbing quality can be (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Conditioning the immune system: New evidence for the modification of physiological responses by drug-associated cues.Marvin D. Krank - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):405-406.
  • “I feel better but I don't know why”: The psychology of implicit emotion regulation.Sander L. Koole & Klaus Rothermund - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (3):389-399.
  • On demonstrating that conditioned immunomodulation is conditioned.Wolfgang Klosterhalfen & Sibylle Klosterhalfen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):404-405.
  • Conditioning of immunosuppression in the treatment of transplant tissue rejection.H. D. Kimmel - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):404-404.
  • A conceptual framework for the neurobiological study of resilience.Raffael Kalisch, Marianne B. Müller & Oliver Tüscher - 2015 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38:1-49.
  • Weakness of Will and the Free-Rider Problem.Jon Elster - 1985 - Economics and Philosophy 1 (2):231-265.
    The study of intrapersonal economic relations, or economics , is still at the programmatic stage. There is no generally accepted paradigm, or even as well-defined set of problems that constitute it as a subdiscipline within economics. Some questions are, however, emerging as foci of interest for a small but increasing number of writers, not just in economics, but also in psychology and philosophy. The writings of Thomas Schelling on self-management, of George Ainslie on self-control, and of Derik Parfit on personal (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • Conditioned immunosuppression and the adaptive function of Pavlovian conditioning.Riley E. Hinson - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):403-403.
  • Avoidance theory: Old wine, older bottles, a few new labels.Philip N. Hineline - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):679-680.
  • Pavlovian conditioned responses: Some elusive results and an indeterminate explanation.Leonard Green - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):402-403.
  • Using a Feedback Environment to Improve Creative Performance: A Dynamic Affect Perspective.Zhenxing Gong & Na Zhang - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Heavy-Tailed Valence Hypothesis: The human capacity for vast variation in pleasure/pain and how to test it.Andrés Gómez-Emilsson & Chris Percy - 2023 - Frontiers in Psychology 14:1127221.
    Introduction: Wellbeing policy analysis is often criticized for requiring a cardinal interpretation of measurement scales, such as ranking happiness on an integer scale from 0-10. The commonly-used scales also implicitly constrain the human capacity for experience, typically that our most intense experiences can only be at most ten times more intense than our mildest experiences. This paper presents the alternative “heavy-tailed valence” (HTV) hypothesis: the notion that the accessible human capacity for emotional experiences of pleasure and pain spans a minimum (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark