Results for 'stimulus-differentiation'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  20
    The effect of stress on stimulus differentiation.Eli Saltz & Winifred J. Riach - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (6):588.
  2.  31
    EEG Differentiation Analysis and Stimulus Set Meaningfulness.Armand Mensen, William Marshall & Giulio Tononi - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    A set of images can be considered as meaningfully different for an observer if they can be distinguished phenomenally from one another. Each phenomenal difference must be supported by some neurophysiological differences. Differentiation analysis aims to quantify neurophysiological differentiation evoked by a given set of stimuli to assess its meaningfulness to the individual observer. As a proof of concept using high-density EEG, we show increased neurophysiological differentiation for a set of natural, meaningfully different images in contrast to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  3.  13
    Differential instrumental conditioning as a function of percentage and amount of positive stimulus reward.James H. McHose & Douglas P. Peters - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 100 (2):413.
  4.  21
    From stimulus-driven to appraisal-driven attention: Towards differential effects of goal relevance and goal relatedness on attention?Audric Mazzietti, Virginie Sellem & Olivier Koenig - 2014 - Cognition and Emotion 28 (8):1483-1492.
    The Component Process Model posits that attention is appraisal-driven rather than stimulus-driven and that the appraisal of relevance is of critical importance in such a mechanism. This means that any stimulus can attract attention or not depending on how relevant it is appraised. This hypothesis was tested in an implicit border similarity judgement task, in which thirsty participants were presented with bottles and vases that were respectively very relevant and weakly relevant to their goal to quench their thirst. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  5.  19
    Differential effects of stimulus frequency and graphic configuration in free- and forced-choice experiments.Warren E. Foote & Leston L. Havens - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (3):340.
  6.  17
    Anxiety, stimulus generalization, and differential conditioning: a comparison of two theories.Frank Restle & Robert S. Beecroft - 1955 - Psychological Review 62 (6):433-437.
  7.  12
    Differentiating aversive conditioning in bistable perception: Avoidance of a percept vs. salience of a stimulus.Gregor Wilbertz & Philipp Sterzer - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 61:38-48.
  8.  21
    Differential relation of latency and response vigor to stimulus similarity in brightness discrimination.Alfred Castaneda & Leonard Worell - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (4):309.
  9.  13
    Differential effects of stimulus and response isolation in paired associate learning.Raymond L. Erickson - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (3):317.
  10.  19
    The effect of differential onset time on the conditioned response strength to elements of a stimulus complex.Delos D. Wickens, Robert S. Gehman & Shirley N. Sullivan - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 58 (1):85.
  11.  21
    Differential eyelid conditioning as a function of stimulus similarity and strength of response to the CS.Malcolm D. Gynther - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (6):408.
  12.  13
    Differential conditioning along two dimensions and stimulus generalization of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response.John W. Moore & Frederick W. Mis - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1 (2):123-125.
  13.  25
    Effects of stimulus complexity, interstimulus interval, and masking task conditions in differential eyelid conditioning.Melanie J. Mayer & Leonard E. Ross - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (3):469.
  14.  23
    "Appropriateness" of the stimulus-reinforcement contingency in instrumental differential conditioning of the eyelid response to the arithmetic concepts of "right" and "wrong".Robert A. Fleming, Louise E. Cerekwicki & David A. Grant - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (2):295.
  15.  20
    Cortisol and stimulus-induced arousal level differentially impact memory for items and backgrounds.Katherine R. Mickley Steinmetz, Arden J. Anderson, Kaci L. Brasher & Thomas S. Brehmer - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (2).
  16.  7
    Bodily arousal differentially impacts stimulus processing and memory: Norepinephrine in interoception.Hugo D. Critchley & Sarah N. Garfinkel - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  15
    The effect of differential overtraining of the positive and negative stimulus on the aversiveness of the negative stimulus.Isaac Behar - 1974 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 3 (2):112-114.
  18.  20
    Compound stimuli in verbal learning: Cognitive and sensory differentiation versus stimulus selection.Eli Saltz - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (1):1.
  19. The reward and punishment responsivity and motivation questionnaire (RPRM-Q): A stimulus-independent self-report measure of reward and punishment sensitivity that differentiates between responsivity and motivation.Nienke C. Jonker, Marieke E. Timmerman & Peter J. de Jong - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Reward and punishment sensitivity seem important traits in understanding behavior in general and psychopathology in particular. Though the definitions used for reward and punishment sensitivity differentiate between responsivity and motivation, the measures thus far used to assess these constructs do not. Further, specificity of the type of reward and punishment in questionnaires might result in measurement bias especially when examining the relationship with psychopathology. Therefore, we developed a stimulus-independent multidimensional questionnaire of reward and punishment sensitivity that differentiates between responsivity (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  14
    Effects of masking tasks on differential eyelid conditioning: A distinction between knowledge of stimulus contingencies and attentional or cognitive activities involving them.Michael N. Nelson & Leonard E. Ross - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (1):1.
  21.  15
    Temporal differentiation and recognition memory for visual stimuli in rhesus monkeys.Mildred Mason & Martha Wilson - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (3):383.
  22.  8
    Associative and differentiation variables in all-or-none learning.Clessen J. Martin - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (3):308.
  23.  4
    A differentiated look at emotions: association between gaze behaviour during the processing of affective videos and emotional granularity.Jonas Potthoff, Albert Wabnegger & Anne Schienle - 2023 - Cognition and Emotion 37 (8):1349-1356.
    The ability to distinguish between subtle differences among emotions of similar valence is labelled emotion differentiation (ED). Previous research has demonstrated that people high in ED are less likely to use disengagement regulation strategies (i.e. avoidance/distraction) during negative affective states.The present eye-tracking study examined associations between ED and visual attention/avoidance of affective stimuli. A total of 160 participants viewed emotional video clips (positive/ negative), which were concurrently presented with a non-affective distractor image. After each video, participants verbally described their (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  21
    Differential eyelid conditioning as a function of the frequency and intensity of auditory CSs.John W. Moore - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (3):250.
  25.  9
    Conceptual recombination and stimulus-independence in non-human animals.Laura Danón - 2022 - Theoria: Revista de Teoría, Historia y Fundamentos de la Ciencia 37 (3):309-330.
    Camp (2009) distinguishes two varieties of conceptual recombination. One of them is full-blown or (as I prefer to call it) spontaneous recombination. The other is causal-counterfactual recombination. She suggests that while human animals recombine their concepts in a full-blown way, many non-human animals are capable of conceptual recombinability but only of the causal-counterfactual kind. In this paper, I argue that there is conceptual space to draw further sub-distinctions on how different animals may recombine their concepts. More specifically, I propose to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  26.  12
    Conceptual recombination and stimulus-independence in non-human animals.Laura Danón - 2022 - Theoria: Revista de Teoría, Historia y Fundamentos de la Ciencia 37 (3):309-330.
    Camp (2009) distinguishes two varieties of conceptual recombination. One of them is full-blown or (as I prefer to call it) spontaneous recombination. The other is causal-counterfactual recombination. She suggests that while human animals recombine their concepts in a full-blown way, many non-human animals are capable of conceptual recombinability but only of the causal-counterfactual kind. In this paper, I argue that there is conceptual space to draw further sub-distinctions on how different animals may recombine their concepts. More specifically, I propose to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  15
    The semantic differential and mediated generalization as measures of meaning.Leonard Lipton & Richard L. Blanton - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 54 (6):431.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  7
    Working Memory Performance for Differentially Conditioned Stimuli.Richard T. Ward, Salahadin Lotfi, Daniel M. Stout, Sofia Mattson, Han-Joo Lee & Christine L. Larson - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Previous work suggests that threat-related stimuli are stored to a greater degree in working memory compared to neutral stimuli. However, most of this research has focused on stimuli with physically salient threat attributes, failing to account for how a “neutral” stimulus that has acquired threat-related associations through differential aversive conditioning influences working memory. The current study examined how differentially conditioned safe and threat stimuli are stored in working memory relative to a novel, non-associated stimuli. Participants completed a differential fear (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  31
    Verbal instructions targeting valence alter negative conditional stimulus evaluations.Camilla C. Luck & Ottmar V. Lipp - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (1):61-80.
    Negative conditional stimulus valence acquired during fear conditioning may enhance fear relapse and is difficult to remove as it extinguishes slowly and does not respond to the instruction that unconditional stimulus presentations will cease. We examined whether instructions targeting CS valence would be more effective. In Experiment 1, an image of one person was paired with an aversive US, while another was presented alone. After acquisition, participants were given positive information about the CS+ poser and negative information about (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  30.  65
    Traces of Identity In Deleuze’s Differential Ontology.Gavin Rae - 2014 - International Journal of Philosophical Studies 22 (1):86-105.
    Deleuze’s differential ontology is a sustained attempt to think and affirm difference as opposed to the unity of identity he insists philosophical thought has tended to privilege. However, by distinguishing between three senses of identity, termed identity of the identical, same, and common, I show that, while Deleuze’s differential ontology offers a powerful critique of identity in the senses of the identical and same, at numerous points in his analysis, such as the virtual-actual movement, the transcendental conditions defining different forms (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  31.  4
    Learning of affective meaning: revealing effects of stimulus pairing and stimulus exposure.Bruno Richter & Mandy Hütter - 2021 - Cognition and Emotion 35 (8):1588-1606.
    Charles E. Osgood's theory of affective meaning defines affect as interplay of three meaning dimensions – evaluation, potency, and activity – that represent the central constituents of our affective ecology. Based on a rigorous Brunswikian sampling procedure, we selected a representative set of stimuli that mirror this ecology. A germane informative analysis explicates and corroborates the sampling approach. We then report two experiments testing whether these dimensions of affective meaning can be learnt by means of stimulus pairing and (...) exposure. Our findings yield evidence for (1) stimulus pairing effects on evaluation and activity, and (2) stimulus exposure effects on potency and activity. Overall, the findings reveal that stimulus pairing and stimulus exposure differentially influence the learning of dimensions of affective meaning. We discuss implications of this research for current emotion theories as well as its contribution to research in the cognition–emotion interface. Finally, we argue that the implementation of representative design by virtue of Brunswikian sampling promotes theory development and opens new research avenues for an original and creative science of cognition and emotion. (shrink)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  15
    Cell‐cycle‐regulatory elements and the control of cell differentiation in the budding yeast.Curt Wittenberg & Roberto La Valle - 2003 - Bioessays 25 (9):856-867.
    The stable differentiation of cells into other cell types typically involves dramatic reorganization of cellular structures and functions. This often includes remodeling of the cell cycle and the apparatus that controls it. Here we review our understanding of the role and regulation of cell cycle control elements during cell differentiation in the yeast, Saccharomyces cerevisiae. Although the process of differentiation may be more overtly obvious in metazoan organisms, those systems are by nature more difficult to study at (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  14
    Cognitive processes in the differential trace conditioning of electrodermal and vasomotor activity.Paul E. Baer & Marcus J. Fuhrer - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 84 (1):176.
  34.  44
    Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization.Harley M. Hanson - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 58 (5):321.
  35. Hypotheses on the Unity and Differentiation of Cultures: Patterns of Architectural Development in Monsoon Asia.Senake Bandaranayake - 1980 - Diogenes 28 (111):65-82.
    One of the major problems (or sometimes pseudo-problems) that archaeologists and historians encounter in the study of ancient cultures is the need to differentiate and to identify the sources of the various concepts, techniques, institutions, forms, designs, motifs, etc., that, at any given moment of time, form the constituent elements of the culture or cultural product to which they have turned their attention; or—to pose the question in its proper framework—to analyse the process of cultural formation inherent in the subject (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  19
    The effects of differential rewards on discrimination reversal learning by monkeys.Donald R. Meyer - 1951 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 41 (4):268.
  37.  30
    Transfer of differential eyelid conditioning: Effects of semantic and formal features of verbal stimuli.Michael J. Zajano, David A. Grant & Marian Schwartz - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (6):1147.
  38.  15
    Varied functions of punishment in differential instrumental conditioning.George T. Taylor - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (2):298.
  39.  16
    Biologically primed acquisition of aversions and association of expected stimulus pairs: Two different forms of learning.Alfons Hamm - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):301-302.
    The present commentary emphasizes that the acquisition of fear always involves complex changes in several quasi-independent response systems. Stimulus-specific electrodermal response differentiation as well as the bias to overestimate the belongingness of certain stimulus pairs mainly indicates cognitive processes of selective orienting and attention. Emotion, however, also involves the activation of subcortical motivational circuits. Why certain stimuli acquire rapid access to these basic motivational systems is not explained by the expectancy bias model.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40. Using illusory line motion to differentiate misrepresentation (stalinesque) and misremembering (orwellian) accounts of consciousness.John Barresi & John R. Christie - 2002 - Consciousness and Cognition 11 (2):347-365.
    It has been suggested that the difference between misremembering (Orwellian) and misrepresentation (Stalinesque) models of consciousness cannot be differentiated (Dennett, 1991). According to an Orwellian account a briefly presented stimulus is seen and then forgotten; whereas, by a Stalinesque account it is never seen. At the same time, Dennett suggested a method for assessing whether an individual is conscious of something. An experiment was conducted which used the suggested method for assessing consciousness to look at Stalinesque and Orwellian distinctions. (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  41.  8
    Comment: The Methodological and Conceptual Utility of Differentiating Emotional Arousal.Dean Sabatinelli - 2016 - Emotion Review 8 (1):81-82.
    The concept of psychophysiological arousal as a component of emotional behavior has a long history, but has not attracted the research attention paid to valence in the burgeoning field of affective neuroscience. The potency of emotional stimuli is often poorly balanced in studies designed to assess appetitive and aversive stimulus processing, and thus I applaud Picard and colleagues’ choice to highlight the arousal dimension of emotional behavior. Any attempt to understand the nature of human emotion must carefully balance the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42.  37
    Arithmetic correctness as the discriminandum in classical and differential eyelid conditioning.Robert A. Fleming, David A. Grant & Jane A. North - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (2):286.
  43.  21
    The ore in simultaneous and differential reversal: Acquisition task, acquisition criterion, and reversal task.Sally E. Sperling - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 84 (2):349.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  24
    Contingency awareness in a symptom learning paradigm: Necessary but not sufficient?Stephan Devriese, Winnie Winters, Ilse Van Diest & Omer Van den Bergh - 2004 - Consciousness and Cognition 13 (3):439-452.
    In previous studies, we found that bodily symptoms can be learned in a differential conditioning paradigm, using odors as conditioned stimuli and CO2-enriched air as unconditioned stimulus . However, this only occurred when the odor CS had a negative valence , and tended to be more pronounced in persons scoring high for Negative Affectivity . This paper considers the necessity and/or sufficiency of awareness of the CS–US contingency in three studies using this paradigm. The relation between contingency awareness and (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  21
    The relation between consciousness and attention: An empirical study using the priming paradigm.Eva Van den Bussche, Gethin Hughes, Nathalie Van Humbeeck & Bert Reynvoet - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (1):86-97.
    6 and 14 recently proposed taxonomies that distinguish between four processing states, based on bottom-up stimulus strength and top-down attentional amplification. The aim of the present study was to empirically test these processing states using the priming paradigm. Our results showed that attention and stimulus strength significantly modulated priming effects: either receiving top-down attention or possessing sufficient bottom-up strength was a prerequisite for a stimulus to elicit priming. When both top-down attention and sufficient bottom-up strength were present, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  46.  38
    Causality and independence in perfectly adapted dynamical systems.Joris M. Mooij & Tineke Blom - 2023 - Journal of Causal Inference 11 (1).
    Perfect adaptation in a dynamical system is the phenomenon that one or more variables have an initial transient response to a persistent change in an external stimulus but revert to their original value as the system converges to equilibrium. With the help of the causal ordering algorithm, one can construct graphical representations of dynamical systems that represent the causal relations between the variables and the conditional independences in the equilibrium distribution. We apply these tools to formulate sufficient graphical conditions (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  47.  20
    Order Matters! Influences of Linear Order on Linguistic Category Learning.Dorothée B. Hoppe, Jacolien van Rij, Petra Hendriks & Michael Ramscar - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (11):e12910.
    Linguistic category learning has been shown to be highly sensitive to linear order, and depending on the task, differentially sensitive to the information provided by preceding category markers (premarkers, e.g., gendered articles) or succeeding category markers (postmarkers, e.g., gendered suffixes). Given that numerous systems for marking grammatical categories exist in natural languages, it follows that a better understanding of these findings can shed light on the factors underlying this diversity. In two discriminative learning simulations and an artificial language learning experiment, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  48.  49
    Neuroimaging techniques for memory detection: Scientific, ethical, and legal issues.Daniel V. Meegan - 2008 - American Journal of Bioethics 8 (1):9 – 20.
    There is considerable interest in the use of neuroimaging techniques for forensic purposes. Memory detection techniques, including the well-publicized Brain Fingerprinting technique (Brain Fingerprinting Laboratories, Inc., Seattle WA), exploit the fact that the brain responds differently to sensory stimuli to which it has been exposed before. When a stimulus is specifically associated with a crime, the resulting brain activity should differentiate between someone who was present at the crime and someone who was not. This article reviews the scientific literature (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  49.  35
    The preparatory set: a novel approach to understanding stress, trauma, and the bodymind therapies.Peter Payne & Mardi A. Crane-Godreau - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9:128767.
    Basic to all motile life is a differential approach/avoid response to perceived features of environment. The stages of response are initial reflexive noticing and orienting to the stimulus, preparation, and execution of response. Preparation involves a coordination of many aspects of the organism: muscle tone, posture, breathing, autonomic functions, motivational/emotional state, attentional orientation and expectations. The organism organizes itself in relation to the challenge. We propose to call this the “preparatory set” (PS). We suggest that the concept of the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  50.  13
    Order Matters! Influences of Linear Order on Linguistic Category Learning.Dorothée B. Hoppe, Jacolien Rij, Petra Hendriks & Michael Ramscar - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (11):e12910.
    Linguistic category learning has been shown to be highly sensitive to linear order, and depending on the task, differentially sensitive to the information provided by preceding category markers (premarkers, e.g., gendered articles) or succeeding category markers (postmarkers, e.g., gendered suffixes). Given that numerous systems for marking grammatical categories exist in natural languages, it follows that a better understanding of these findings can shed light on the factors underlying this diversity. In two discriminative learning simulations and an artificial language learning experiment, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000