Results for 'Representations of spatiality'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Mental representations of spatial relations-combining recognition times and distance estimates.Kf Wender & M. Wagener - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (6):507-507.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  49
    Internalized constraints in the representation of spatial layout.Helene Intraub - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (4):677-678.
    Shepard's (1994) choice of kinematic geometry to support his theory is questioned by Todorovic, Schwartz, and Hecht. His theoretical framework, however, can be applied to another domain that may be less susceptible to some of their concerns. The domain is the representation of spatial layout. [Hecht; Schwartz; Shepard; Todorovic].
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  55
    The neural representation of spatial predicate-argument structures in sign language.Bencie Woll - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (3):300-301.
    Evidence from studies of the processing of topographic and classifier constructions in sign language sentences provides a model of how a mental scene description can be represented linguistically, but it also raises questions about how this can be related to spatial linguistic descriptions in spoken languages and their processing. This in turn provides insights into models of the evolution of language.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  14
    Augmented Aesthetics in the Representation of Spatial Atmosphere.Fatma İpek Ek - 2022 - Environment, Space, Place 14 (2):83-103.
    Abstract:Atmosphere in architecture acts as a communication tool between the space and its experiencers. This tool has the potential of being detached from the physical environment and conveyed by memories and imagination, which may augment the physical environment in a poetic way. This paper aims to demonstrate this potential by utilizing the technique of comparative reading using unmanipulated photographs of physical space, computer- generated film/images of the same space, and a spatial narration/text, all in the context of Japanese architecture. By (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  23
    Visual Analysis and Representation of Spatial Relations.R. J. Watt - 1990 - Mind and Language 5 (4):267-288.
  6.  58
    Body-specific representations of spatial location.Tad T. Brunyé, Aaron Gardony, Caroline R. Mahoney & Holly A. Taylor - 2012 - Cognition 123 (2):229-239.
  7. Young Children's Representations of Spatial and Functional Relations Between Objects.Rachel Keen & Elizabeth S. Spelke - unknown
    Three experiments investigated changes from 15 to 30 months of age in children’s (N = 114) mastery of relations between an object and an aperture, supporting surface, or form. When choosing between objects to insert into an aperture, older children selected objects of an appropriate size and shape, but younger children showed little selectivity. Further experiments probed the sources of younger children’s difficulty by comparing children’s performance placing a target object in a hole, on a 2-dimensional form, or atop another (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  8.  58
    Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect.E. Rusconi, B. Kwan, B. Giordano, C. Umilta & B. Butterworth - 2006 - Cognition 99 (2):113-129.
  9.  39
    Spatial Representation of Ordinal Information.Meng Zhang, Xuefei Gao, Baichen Li, Shuyuan Yu, Tianwei Gong, Ting Jiang, Qingfen Hu & Yinghe Chen - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10. Parts and Places: The Structures of Spatial Representation.Roberto Casati & Achille C. Varzi - 1999 - MIT Press.
    Thinking about space is thinking about spatial things. The table is on the carpet; hence the carpet is under the table. The vase is in the box; hence the box is not in the vase. But what does it mean for an object to be somewhere? How are objects tied to the space they occupy? This book is concerned with these and other fundamental issues in the philosophy of spatial representation. Our starting point is an analysis of the interplay between (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   206 citations  
  11.  26
    Listeners use speaker identity to access representations of spatial perspective during online language comprehension.Rachel A. Ryskin, Ranxiao Frances Wang & Sarah Brown-Schmidt - 2016 - Cognition 147 (C):75-84.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  12.  19
    Spatial representation of objects in the young blind child.Barbara Landau - 1991 - Cognition 38 (2):145-178.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  13.  20
    Spatial representations of numbers and letters in children.Jan Lonnemann, Janosch Linkersdörfer, Telse Nagler, Marcus Hasselhorn & Sven Lindberg - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  14. Nonlinear effects of spatial connectedness implicate hierarchically structured representations in visual working memory.Błażej Skrzypulec & Adam Chuderski - 2020 - Journal of Memory and Language 113:104124.
    Five experiments investigated the role of spatial connectedness between a pair of objects presented in the change detection task for the actual capacity of visual working memory (VWM) in healthy young adults (total N = 405). Three experiments yielded a surprising nonlinear relationship between the proportion of pair-wise connected objects and capacity, with the highest capacity observed for homogenous displays, when either all objects were connected or disjointed. A drop in capacity, ranging from an average of a quarter of an (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  25
    An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time.Antonino Vallesi, Malcolm A. Binns & Tim Shallice - 2008 - Cognition 107 (2):501-527.
  16.  27
    The Relational SNARC: Spatial Representation of Nonsymbolic Ratios.Rui Meng, Percival G. Matthews & Elizabeth Y. Toomarian - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (8):e12778.
    Recent research in numerical cognition has begun to systematically detail the ability of humans and nonhuman animals to perceive the magnitudes of nonsymbolic ratios. These relationally defined analogs to rational numbers offer new potential insights into the nature of human numerical processing. However, research into their similarities with and connections to symbolic numbers remains in its infancy. The current research aims to further explore these similarities by investigating whether the magnitudes of nonsymbolic ratios are associated with space just as symbolic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  17.  29
    The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized.Wim Gevers, Bert Reynvoet & Wim Fias - 2003 - Cognition 87 (3):B87-B95.
  18.  39
    The role of spatial boundaries in shaping long-term event representations.Aidan J. Horner, James A. Bisby, Aijing Wang, Katrina Bogus & Neil Burgess - 2016 - Cognition 154 (C):151-164.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  19.  46
    The effects of spatial language on spatial representation: Setting some boundaries.Edward Munnich & Barbara Landau - 2003 - In Dedre Gentner & Susan Goldin-Meadow (eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. MIT Press. pp. 113--155.
  20.  20
    Comparative cognition of spatial representation.Donald M. Wilkie & Robert J. Wilison - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):97-98.
  21. Towards a Typology of Spatial Representations.Petronia Petrar - 2011 - International Journal on Humanistic Ideology 4 (1):49-65.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  50
    Time drawings: Spatial representation of temporal concepts.María Juliana Leone, Alejo Salles, Alejandro Pulver, Diego Andrés Golombek & Mariano Sigman - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 59:10-25.
  23. Infinity and givenness: Kant on the intuitive origin of spatial representation.Daniel Smyth - 2014 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 44 (5-6):551-579.
    I advance a novel interpretation of Kant's argument that our original representation of space must be intuitive, according to which the intuitive status of spatial representation is secured by its infinitary structure. I defend a conception of intuitive representation as what must be given to the mind in order to be thought at all. Discursive representation, as modelled on the specific division of a highest genus into species, cannot account for infinite complexity. Because we represent space as infinitely complex, the (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  24.  27
    Spatial programming and the representation of salience.Jay A. Edelman, Jacqueline Gottlieb & Michael E. Goldberg - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):682-682.
    The posterior parietal cortex and frontal eye field contain maps of visual salience on which the decision to choose a saccade may be based. However, an averaging express saccade is not represented by a victorious unimodal representation in the superior colliculus. Normalization as described by Findlay & Walker is not necessary for the generation of saccades.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  10
    The Development of Spatial Representation Through Teaching Block-Building in Kindergartners.Liman Cai, Jiutong Luo, Hui Zhang & Jinling Ying - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26.  46
    The representation selection problem: Why we should favor the geometric-module framework of spatial reorientation over the view-matching framework.Alexandre Duval - 2019 - Cognition 192 (C):103985.
    Many species rely on the three-dimensional surface layout of an environment to find a desired goal following disorientation. They generally do so to the exclusion of other important spatial cues. Two influential frameworks for explaining that phenomenon are provided by geometric-module theories and view-matching theories of reorientation respectively. The former posit a module that operates only on representations of the global geo- metry of three-dimensional surfaces to guide behavior. The latter place snapshots, stored representations of the subject’s two-dimensional (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27.  29
    Anticipatory spatial representation of 3D regions explored by sighted observers and a deaf-and-blind-observer.Helene Intraub - 2004 - Cognition 94 (1):19-37.
  28.  8
    Spatial versus graphical representation of distributional semantic knowledge.Shufan Mao, Philip A. Huebner & Jon A. Willits - 2024 - Psychological Review 131 (1):104-137.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  9
    Idiosyncratic spatial representations of the days of the week in individuals without synesthesia.Shogo Makioka - 2021 - Cognition 207 (C):104500.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30. The role of spatial representations in complex problem solving.Lynn A. Cooper - 1988 - In Stephen R. Schiffer & Susan Steele (eds.), Cognition and Representation. Westview Press. pp. 53--86.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  2
    Wayfinding: an interdisciplinary journey into the domain of spatial representation in philosophy and psychology.Bert Karcher - 2011 - Paderborn: Mentis.
    "This wholeheartedly interdisciplinary book explores the possibility of domain specific cooperation between philosophy and psychology concerning questions on spatial representation. Its leitmotif is the importance of movement in concord with the workings of the body schema. Against the background of embodiment, situatedness, and Susan Hurley's notion of a ninety-degree shift it is spelled out how true, domain specific cooperation between the disciplines can be accomplished. By enriching Grush's naturalistic account of representation (emulation theory) with insights stemming from teleosemantics, the notion (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  19
    A qualitative spatial representation of string loops as holes.Pedro Cabalar & Paulo E. Santos - 2016 - Artificial Intelligence 238 (C):1-10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  39
    Spatial language as a window on representations of three-dimensional space.Kevin J. Holmes & Phillip Wolff - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):550-551.
    Recent research investigating the language–thought interface in the spatial domain points to representations of the horizontal and vertical dimensions that closely resemble those posited by Jeffery et al. However, the findings suggest that such representations, rather than being tied to navigation, may instead reflect more general properties of the perception of space.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  34
    Parts and Places: The Structures of Spatial Representation.Franklin Mason, Roberto Casati & Achille C. Varzi - 2001 - Philosophical Review 110 (3):479.
    The purpose of Parts and Places, say Casati and Varzi in their introduction, is to construct “a theory of our spatial competence,” a theory that will lay bare how we conceive of space and the things that lie within it. Its purpose, then, is psychological, not metaphysical. Its object of study is not space. It is not the things that lie within it. Rather its object of study is us. In this regard, Parts and Places is at best a mixed (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  35.  44
    The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex.J. F. Stein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):691-700.
    The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is the most likely site where egocentric spatial relationships are represented in the brain. PPC cells receive visual, auditory, somaesthetic, and vestibular sensory inputs; oculomotor, head, limb, and body motor signals; and strong motivational projections from the limbic system. Their discharge increases not only when an animal moves towards a sensory target, but also when it directs its attention to it. PPC lesions have the opposite effect: sensory inattention and neglect. The PPC does not seem (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   87 citations  
  36.  36
    Using representations of space to study early modern physical science: An example of philosophy in the service of history.David Marshall Miller - manuscript
    Most historians of science eagerly acknowledge that the early modern period witnessed a shift from a prevailing Aristotelian, spherical, centered conception of space to a prevailing Cartesian, rectilinear, oriented spatial framework. Indeed, this shift underlay many of the important advances for which the period is celebrated. However, historians have failed to engage the general conceptual shift, focusing instead on the particular explanatory developments that resulted. This historical lacuna can be attributed to a historiographical problem: the lack of an adequate unit (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  26
    Simulating a lesion in a basis function model of spatial representations: Comparison with hemineglect.Alexandre Pouget & Terrence J. Sejnowski - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (3):653-673.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  38.  39
    Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys.Carol L. Krumhansl & Edward J. Kessler - 1982 - Psychological Review 89 (4):334-368.
  39.  40
    Affect biases memory of location: Evidence for the spatial representation of affect.L. Elizabeth Crawford, Skye M. Margolies, John T. Drake & Meghan E. Murphy - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (8):1153-1169.
  40.  56
    Electrocorticographic representations of segmental features in continuous speech.Fabien Lotte, Jonathan S. Brumberg, Peter Brunner, Aysegul Gunduz, Anthony L. Ritaccio, Cuntai Guan & Gerwin Schalk - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9:119171.
    Acoustic speech output results from coordinated articulation of dozens of muscles, bones and cartilages of the vocal mechanism. While we commonly take the fluency and speed of our speech productions for granted, the neural mechanisms facilitating the requisite muscular control are not completely understood. Previous neuroimaging and electrophysiology studies of speech sensorimotor control has typically concentrated on speech sounds (i.e., phonemes, syllables and words) in isolation; sentence-length investigations have largely been used to inform coincident linguistic processing. In this study, we (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  41.  15
    Spatial imaginary in recent Chilean narrative: the aquarium as representation of intimacy in Contreras's, Zambra's and Bolaño’s novels.Macarena Areco Morales - 2014 - Alpha (Osorno) 38:9-22.
    El objetivo de este trabajo es analizar el acuario como una figuración espacial del imaginario social en tres novelas escritas por autores chilenos en las últimas dos décadas, El nadador de Gonzalo Contreras, La vida privada de los árboles de Alejandro Zambra y Monsieur Pain de Roberto Bolaño. Su finalidad es atisbar en la configuración imaginaria de los espacios de la intimidad y de la intemperie en la posdictadura chilena y, más generalmente, en el entorno global en que se desarrolla (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42. The view from here: The nonsymbolic structure of spatial representation.Patricia S. Churchland, Ilya B. Farber & Will Peterman - 2001 - In João Branquinho (ed.), The Foundations of Cognitive Science. Oxford University Press UK.
  43. Unilateral neglect and the objectivity of spatial representation.Bill Brewer - 1992 - Mind and Language 7 (3):222-39.
    Patients may show a more-or-less complete deviation of the head and eyes towards the right (ipsilesional) side [that is, to the same side of egocentric space as the brain lesion responsible for their disorder]. If addressed by the examiner from the left (contralesional) side [the opposite side to their lesion], patients with severe extrapersonal neglect may fail to respond or may look for the speaker in the right side of the room, turning head and eyes more and more to the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  44.  37
    Sex differences in the spatial representation of number.Rebecca Bull, Alexandra A. Cleland & Thomas Mitchell - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (1):181.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  45.  53
    Implicit short-lived motor representations of space in brain damaged and healthy subjects.Yves Rossetti - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):520-558.
    This article reviews experimental evidence for a specific sensorimotor function which can be dissociated from higher level representations of space. It attempts to delineate this function on the basis of results obtained by psychophysical experiments performed with brain damaged and healthy subjects. Eye and hand movement control exhibit automatic features, such that they are incompatible with conscious control. In addition, they rely on a reference frame different from the one used by conscious perception. Neuropsychological cases provide a strong support (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  46.  52
    Parts and places: The structures of spatial representation.Franklin Mason - 2001 - Philosophical Review 110 (3):479-481.
    The purpose of Parts and Places, say Casati and Varzi in their introduction, is to construct “a theory of our spatial competence,” a theory that will lay bare how we conceive of space and the things that lie within it. Its purpose, then, is psychological, not metaphysical. Its object of study is not space. It is not the things that lie within it. Rather its object of study is us. In this regard, Parts and Places is at best a mixed (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  14
    The future is in front, to the right, or below: Development of spatial representations of time in three dimensions.Ariel Starr & Mahesh Srinivasan - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104603.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  48.  11
    Neuromagnetic representation of melodic contour processing in human auditory cortex.Sabrina Taddeo, Martin Schulz, Martin Andermann & André Rupp - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16:909159.
    The pattern of ups and downs in a sequence with varying pitch can be heard as a melodic contour. Contrary to single pitch, the neural representation of melodic contour information in the auditory cortex is rarely investigated, and it is not clear whether the processing entails a hemispheric asymmetry. The present magnetoencephalography study assessed the neuromagnetic responses of N = 18 normal-hearing adults to four-note sequences with fixed vs. varying pitch that were presented either monaurally or diotically; data were analyzed (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  14
    Generalized Lagrangian-Path Representation of Non-Relativistic Quantum Mechanics.Massimo Tessarotto & Claudio Cremaschini - 2016 - Foundations of Physics 46 (8):1022-1061.
    In this paper a new trajectory-based representation to non-relativistic quantum mechanics is formulated. This is ahieved by generalizing the notion of Lagrangian path which lies at the heart of the deBroglie-Bohm “ pilot-wave” interpretation. In particular, it is shown that each LP can be replaced with a statistical ensemble formed by an infinite family of stochastic curves, referred to as generalized Lagrangian paths. This permits the introduction of a new parametric representation of the Schrödinger equation, denoted as GLP-parametrization, and of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  50. Organization of spatial knowledge in children.Herbert L. Pick - 1993 - In Naomi Eilan, Rosaleen A. McCarthy & Bill Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation: problems in philosophy and psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell. pp. 31--42.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000