Results for ' Stimulus-response compatibility'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  28
    Spatial stimulus-response compatibility and affordance effects are not ruled by the same mechanisms.Marianna Ambrosecchia, Barbara F. M. Marino, Luiz G. Gawryszewski & Lucia Riggio - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  2.  13
    Stimulusresponse compatibility based on affective arousal.Thomas Kleinsorge - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (4):663-674.
  3.  13
    Stimulus-response compatibility effect in left-right discriminations.Leslie A. Whitaker - 1980 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 15 (5):345-347.
  4.  20
    Stimulus-response compatibility as a determinant of interference in a Stroop-like task.Elaine Fox - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (5):377-380.
  5.  10
    Strength of auditory stimulus-response compatability as a function of task complexity.James Callan, Diane Klisz & Oscar A. Parsons - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (6):1039.
  6.  15
    Multidimensional vector model of stimulusresponse compatibility.Motonori Yamaguchi & Robert W. Proctor - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (2):272-303.
  7.  23
    Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--A model and taxonomy.Sylvan Kornblum, Thierry Hasbroucq & Allen Osman - 1990 - Psychological Review 97 (2):253-270.
  8.  26
    Flowers and spiders in spatial stimulus-response compatibility: does affective valence influence selection of task-sets or selection of responses?Motonori Yamaguchi, Jing Chen, Scott Mishler & Robert W. Proctor - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (5):1003-1017.
    ABSTRACTThe present study examined the effect of stimulus valence on two levels of selection in the cognitive system, selection of a task-set and selection of a response. In the first experiment, participants performed a spatial compatibility task in which stimulus-response mappings were determined by stimulus valence. There was a standard spatial stimulus-response compatibility effect for positive stimuli and a reversed SRC effect for negative stimuli, but the same data could be interpreted (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  9.  25
    "Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--A model and taxonomy": Correction to Kornblum et al.Sylvan Kornblum, Thierry Hasbroucq & Allen Osman - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (1):44-44.
  10.  46
    Word recognition in the split brain and PET studies of spatial stimulus-response compatibility support contextual integration.Marco Iacoboni - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):690-691.
    The neural substrates of context effects in word perception are still largely unclear. Interhemispheric priming phenomena in word recognition, typically observed in normal subjects, are absent in commissurotomized patients. This suggests that callosal fibers may provide contextual integration. In addition, certain characteristics of human frontal cortical fields subserving sensorimotor learning, as investigated by positron emission tomography, provide evidence for contextual integration not confined to the visual system. This supports the notion of common aspects of cortical computations in different cerebral areas.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  17
    Stimulus uncertainty, response uncertainty, and stimulus-response compatibility as determinants of schizophrenic reaction time performance.D. R. Hemsley - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8 (6):425-427.
  12.  37
    Common valence coding in action and evaluation: Affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli.Andreas B. Eder & Karl Christoph Klauer - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1297-1322.
    A common coding account of bidirectional evaluation–behaviour interactions proposes that evaluative attributes of stimuli and responses are coded in a common representational format. This assumption was tested in two experiments that required evaluations of positive and negative stimuli during the generation of a positively or negatively charged motor response. The results of both experiments revealed a reduced evaluative sensitivity (d′) towards response-compatible stimulus valences. This action–valence blindness supports the notion of a common valence coding in action and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  13.  26
    Stimulus discriminability and S-R compatibility: Evidence for independent effects in choice reaction time.Irving Biederman & Robert Kaplan - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 86 (3):434.
  14.  18
    The effect of stimulus complexity on discrimination responses.Lee W. Gregg - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (4):289.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  26
    Auditory S-R compatibility: Reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence.J. Richard Simon, James V. Hinrichs & John L. Craft - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 86 (1):97.
  16.  81
    S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes.Paul M. Fitts & Charles M. Seeger - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 46 (3):199.
  17.  72
    S-r compatibility: Correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes.Paul M. Fitts & Richard L. Deininger - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (6):483.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  18.  37
    S-R compatability, response discriminability, and response codes in choice reaction time.Harvey G. Shulman & Alan McConkie - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (2):375.
  19.  46
    S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code.Richard J. Wallace - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 88 (3):354.
  20.  33
    S-R compatibility and information reduction.Paul M. Fitts & Irving Biederman - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (4):408.
  21.  18
    Spatial S-R compatibility effects involving kinesthetic cues.Richard J. Wallace - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 93 (1):163.
  22.  35
    Grasping the concept of personal property.Merryn D. Constable, Ada Kritikos & Andrew P. Bayliss - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):430-437.
    The concept of property is integral to personal and societal development, yet understanding of the cognitive basis of ownership is limited. Objects are the most basic form of property, so our physical interactions with owned objects may elucidate nuanced aspects of ownership. We gave participants a coffee mug to decorate, use and keep. The experimenter also designed a mug of her own. In Experiment 1, participants performed natural lifting actions with each mug. Participants lifted the Experimenter’s mug with greater care, (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  23.  34
    Effects of stimulus uncertainty and S-R compatibility on speed of digit coding.Louis D. Costa, Morton Horwitz & Herbert G. Vaughan - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (6):895.
  24.  12
    The Effects of Computer-Based and Motor-Imagery Training on Scoring Ability in Lacrosse.Takahiro Hirao & Hiroaki Masaki - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11:529374.
    Previous studies have confirmed that the temporal attentional control created by the repetition of stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) tasks was transferred to shooting skills in lacrosse players. In the current study, we investigated whether combining motor imagery training with SRC tasks could enhance the scoring ability of lacrosse players. We grouped 33 male lacrosse players into three groups: an SRC task and motor imagery group (referred as to SRC+Image), an SRC task group, and a control group. Players in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  22
    Probability, compatibility, speed, and accuracy.O. Joseph Harm & Joseph S. Lappin - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 100 (2):416.
  26.  21
    S-R compatibility and the relative frequency effect in choice reaction time.Harold L. Hawkins & James R. Underhill - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 91 (2):280.
  27.  56
    Are Automatic Imitation and Spatial Compatibility Mediated by Different Processes?Richard P. Cooper, Caroline Catmur & Cecilia Heyes - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (4):605-630.
    Automatic imitation or “imitative compatibility” is thought to be mediated by the mirror neuron system and to be a laboratory model of the motor mimicry that occurs spontaneously in naturalistic social interaction. Imitative compatibility and spatial compatibility effects are known to depend on different stimulus dimensions—body movement topography and relative spatial position. However, it is not yet clear whether these two types of stimulusresponse compatibility effect are mediated by the same or different cognitive (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  28.  28
    Prediction outcome, S-R compatibility, and choice reaction time.Charles P. Whitman & E. Scott Geller - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 91 (2):299.
  29.  30
    Does subliminal priming of free response choices depend on task set or automatic response activation?Patrick A. O’Connor & W. Trammell Neill - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (2):280-287.
    In a task requiring speeded bidirectional responses to arrow symbols , “free choice” responses to interspersed bidirectional stimuli are influenced by masked directional primes . By varying stimulusresponse compatibility, we tested whether this priming effect is mediated by the conscious instructional set, or instead by pre-existing directional associations to the symbols. In two experiments, one group of participants was instructed to respond with the hand consistent with the implied direction of the arrow symbols, while another group was (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  30.  25
    On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility.Anthony G. Greenwald - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 94 (1):52.
  31.  14
    Anticipatory affect during action preparation: evidence from backward compatibility in dual-task performance.Andreas B. Eder, Roland Pfister, David Dignath & Bernhard Hommel - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (6):1211-1224.
    Upcoming responses in the second of two subsequently performed tasks can speed up compatible responses in the temporally preceding first task. Two experiments extend previous demonstration of such backward compatibility to affective features: responses to affective stimuli were faster in Task 1 when an affectively compatible response effect was anticipated for Task 2. This emotional backward-compatibility effect demonstrates that representations of the affective consequences of the Task 2 response were activated before the selection of a (...) in Task 1 was completed. This finding is problematic for the assumption of a serial stimulus-response translation stage. It also shows that the affective consequence of a response is anticipated during, and has an impact on stimulus-response translation, which implies that action planning considers codes representing and predicting the emotional consequences of actions. Implications for the control of emotional actions are discussed. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  24
    Interactive effect of drive and S-R compatibility on speed of digit coding.Dennis L. Wack & Nickolas B. Cottrell - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (3p1):562.
  33. The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning.Bernhard Hommel, Jochen Müsseler, Gisa Aschersleben & Wolfgang Prinz - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (5):849-878.
    Traditional approaches to human information processing tend to deal with perception and action planning in isolation, so that an adequate account of the perception-action interface is still missing. On the perceptual side, the dominant cognitive view largely underestimates, and thus fails to account for, the impact of action-related processes on both the processing of perceptual information and on perceptual learning. On the action side, most approaches conceive of action planning as a mere continuation of stimulus processing, thus failing to (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   342 citations  
  34.  27
    Temporal and symbolic S-R compatibility in a sequential information-processing task.Richard P. LeMay & J. Richard Simon - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (3p1):558.
  35.  17
    The negative compatibility effect with nonmasking flankers: A case for mask-triggered inhibition hypothesis.Piotr Jaśkowski - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (3):765-777.
    Visual targets which follow a prime stimulus and a mask can be identified faster when they are incompatible rather than compatible with the prime . According to the self-inhibition hypothesis, the initial activation of the motor response is elicited by the prime based on its identity. This activation leads to benefits for compatible trials and costs for incompatible trials. This motor activation is followed by an inhibition phase, leading to an NCE if perceptual evidence of the prime is (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  36.  22
    Compatibility and Time-Sharing in Serial Reaction Time.Steven W. Keele - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (4):529.
  37.  50
    The Simon Effect in Action: Planning and/or On‐Line Control Effects?Claudia Scorolli, Antonello Pellicano, Roberto Nicoletti, Sandro Rubichi & Umberto Castiello - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (5):972-991.
    Choice reaction tasks are performed faster when stimulus location corresponds to response location. This spatial stimulusresponse compatibility effect affects performance at the level of action planning and execution. However, when response selection is completed before movement initiation, the Simon effect arises only at the planning level. The aim of this study was to ascertain whether when a precocious response selection is requested, the Simon effect can be detected on the kinematics characterizing the online (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  38.  62
    Affect and action: Towards an event-coding account.Tristan Lavender & Bernhard Hommel - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1270-1296.
    Viewing emotion from an evolutionary perspective, researchers have argued that simple responses to affective stimuli can be triggered without mediation of cognitive processes. Indeed, findings suggest that positively and negatively valenced stimuli trigger approach and avoidance movements automatically. However, affective stimulusresponse compatibility phenomena share so many central characteristics with nonaffective stimulusresponse compatibility phenomena that one may doubt whether the underlying mechanisms differ. We suggest an “affectively enriched” version of the theory of event coding (TEC) (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  39.  12
    Bright on the right feels right: SQUARC compatibility is hedonically marked.Charlotte S. Löffler, Judith Gerten, Mariam Mamporia, Johanna Müller, Theresa Neu, Julia Rumpf, Miriam Schiller, Yannik Schneider, Mirella Wozniak & Sascha Topolinski - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (4):767-772.
    According to the Spatial Quantity Association of Response Codes (SQUARC), people hold a mental association between horizontal position and quantity (lower quantities left, higher quantities right). While a large body of research has explored this effect for response speed and judgment accuracy, the affective downstream consequences of the SQUARC remain unexplored. Aiming to address this gap, the present two experiments (pre-registered, total N = 521) investigated whether stimulus arrangements that are compatible with the SQUARC for luminance are (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  40.  22
    Perceived Ownership of Avatars Influences Visual Perspective Taking.Christian Böffel & Jochen Müsseler - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:350620.
    Modern computer-based applications often require the user to interact with avatars. Depending on the task at hand, spatial dissociation between the orientations of the user and the avatars might arise. As a consequence, the user has to adopt the avatar's perspective and identify herself/himself with the avatar, possibly changing the user's self-representation in the process. The present study aims to identify the conditions that benefit this change of perspective with objective performance measures and subjective self-estimations by integrating the idea of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  41.  14
    Magnitude and Order are Both Relevant in SNARC and SNARC‐like Effects: A Commentary on Casasanto and Pitt.Valter Prpic, Serena Mingolo, Tiziano Agostini & Mauro Murgia - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (7):e13006.
    In a recent paper by Casasanto and Pitt (2019), the authors addressed a debate regarding the role of order and magnitude in SNARC and SNARC‐like effects. Their position is that all these effects can be explained by order, while magnitude could only account for a subset of evidence. Although we agree that order can probably explain the majority of these effects, in this commentary we argue that magnitude is still relevant, since there is evidence that cannot be explained based on (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  42.  14
    The impact of horizontal and vertical Luminance SNARC compatibility on affective judgments.Beatriz Gusmão, Charlotte S. Löffler & Sascha Topolinski - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (8):1522-1530.
    Research on the Spatial Quantity Association of Response Codes (SQUARC) has documented associations between spatial position and mental representations of quantity. Large quantities are associated with right and top, small quantities are associated with left and bottom. Resulting compatibility effects have largely been documented for response speed and judgment accuracy. Recently, employing luminance as quantity, Löffler et al. (2022) generalised such SQUARC compatibility effects to affective judgments, showing that horizontally SQUARC-compatible stimulus arrangements (i.e. bright on (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  27
    The joint Simon effect depends on perceived agency, but not intentionality, of the alternative action.Anna Stenzel, Thomas Dolk, Lorenza S. Colzato, Roberta Sellaro, Bernhard Hommel & Roman Liepelt - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:96464.
    A co-actor’s intentionality has been suggested to be a key modulating factor for joint action effects like the joint Simon effect (JSE). However, in previous studies intentionality has often been confounded with agency defined as perceiving the initiator of an action as being the causal source of the action. The aim of the present study was to disentangle the role of agency and intentionality as modulating factors of the JSE. In Experiment 1, participants performed a joint go/nogo Simon task next (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  44.  21
    Sequential effects in choice reaction time.Roger W. Schvaneveldt & William G. Chase - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (1):1.
  45.  53
    Dissociation of Processes Underlying Spatial S-R Compatibility: Evidence for the Independent Influence of What and Where.Jeffrey P. Toth, Brian Levine, Donald T. Stuss, Alfred Oh, Gordon Winocur & Nachshon Meiran - 1995 - Consciousness and Cognition 4 (4):483-501.
    The process-dissociation procedure was used to estimate the influence of spatial and form-based processing in the Simon task. Subjects made manual responses to the direction of arrows . The results provide evidence that the form and spatial location of a single stimulus can have functionally independent effects on performance. They also indicate the existence of two kinds of automaticity—an associative component that reflects prior S-R mappings and a nonassociative component that reflects the correspondence between stimulus and response (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  46.  30
    PRP-paradigm provides evidence for a perceptual origin of the negative compatibility effect.Daniel Krüger, Susan Klapötke & Uwe Mattler - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (3):866-881.
    Visual stimuli that are made invisible by masking can affect motor responses to a subsequent target stimulus. When a prime is followed by a mask which is followed by a target stimulus, an inverse priming effect has been found: Responses are slow and frequently incorrect when prime and target stimuli are congruent, but fast and accurate when prime and target stimuli are incongruent. To functionally localize the origins of inverse priming effects, we applied the psychological refractory period paradigm (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  47.  30
    On the Validity of Simulating Stagewise Development by Means of PDP Networks: Application of Catastrophe Analysis and an Experimental Test of Rule‐Like Network Performance.Maartje E. J. Raijmakers, Sylvester von Koten & Peter C. M. Molenaar - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (1):101-136.
    This article addresses the ability of Parallel Distributed Processing (PDP) networks to generate stagewise cognitive development in accordance with Piaget's theory of cognitive epigenesis. We carried out a replication study of the simulation experiments by McClelland (1989) and McClelland and Jenkins (1991) in which a PDP network learns to solve balance scale problems. In objective tests motivated from catastrophe theory, a mathematical theory of transitions in epigenetical systems, no evidence for stage transitions in network performance was found. It is concluded (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  48.  7
    Understanding Imitation in Papio papio: The Role of Experience and the Presence of a Conspecific Demonstrator.Anthony Formaux, Eoin O'Sullivan, Joël Fagot & Nicolas Claidière - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (3):e13117.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 3, March 2022.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  25
    Locus of the relative frequency effect in choice reaction time.Harold L. Hawkins, Stephen L. MacKay, Susan L. Holley, Bruce D. Friedin & Stephen L. Cohen - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 101 (1):90.
  50.  4
    Integrating Social Cognition Into Domain‐General Control: Interactive Activation and Competition for the Control of Action (ICON).Robert Ward & Richard Ramsey - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (2):e13415.
    Social cognition differs from general cognition in its focus on understanding, perceiving, and interpreting social information. However, we argue that the significance of domain‐general processes for controlling cognition has been historically undervalued in social cognition and social neuroscience research. We suggest much of social cognition can be characterized as specialized feature representations supported by domain‐general cognitive control systems. To test this proposal, we develop a comprehensive working model, based on an interactive activation and competition architecture and applied to the control (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 1000