Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Thought and Action in Education.Thomas Aastrup Rømer - 2015 - Educational Philosophy and Theory 47 (3):260-275.
    In much theory there is a tendency to place thought above action, or the opposite, action over thought. The consequence of the first option is that philosophy or scientific evidence gains the upper hand in educational thinking. The consequence of the second view is that pragmatism and relativism become the dominant features. This article discusses how different branches of the Aristotelian tradition can mediate between these two views. I argue, contrary to some other Aristotelian approaches, that thinking and action are (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Re-afference in space and movement perception.Austin H. Riesen - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):78-78.
  • The ‘Spaghettification’ of Performativity Across Cultural Boundaries: The Trans-culturality/Trans-Spatiality of Digital Communication As an Event Horizon for Speech Acts.Mario Ricca - 2022 - International Journal for the Semiotics of Law - Revue Internationale de Sémiotique Juridique 35 (6):2435-2479.
    Recently the CJEU decision in the case of ‘Ewa Glawischnig-Piesczek v. Facebook Ireland Limited’ has raised the issue of the transcultural/trans-territorial signification of hate speech and hate crimes. Taking a cue from this decision and the related semiotic/legal implications, the paper proposes an analysis of the semio/pragmatic conditions for the production of performativity inherent in hate speech across different cultural universes of discourse. Given that web-based digital communication is global—at least, potentially—regardless of any spatial/political compartmentalization, it crosses different semio-cultural circuits. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How to Undo (and Redo) Words with Facts: A Semio-enactivist Approach to Law, Space and Experience.Mario Ricca - 2022 - International Journal for the Semiotics of Law - Revue Internationale de Sémiotique Juridique 36 (1):313-367.
    In this essay both the facts/values and facticity/normativity divides are considered from the perspective of global semiotics and with specific regard to the relationships between legal meaning and spatial scope of law’s experience. Through an examination of the inner and genetic projective significance of categorization, I will analyze the semantic dynamics of the descriptive parts comprising legal sentences in order to show the intermingling of factual and axiological/teleological categorizations in the unfolding of legal experience. Subsequently, I will emphasize the translational (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cultures in Orbit, or Justi-fying Differences in Cosmic Space: On Categorization, Territorialization and Rights Recognition.Mario Ricca - 2018 - International Journal for the Semiotics of Law - Revue Internationale de Sémiotique Juridique 31 (4):829-875.
    The many constraints of outer space experience challenge the human ability to coexist. Paradoxically, astronauts assert that on the international space station there are no conflicts or, at least, that they are able to manage their differences, behavioral as well as cognitive, in full respect of human rights and the imperatives of cooperative living. The question is: Why? Why in those difficult, a-terrestrial, and therefore almost unnatural conditions do human beings seem to be able to peacefully and collaboratively live together? (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The formal and the opaque.Georges Rey - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):90-92.
  • Information pickup is the activity of perceiving.Edward S. Reed - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):397-398.
  • Attention as an explanatory concept in perceptual adaptation.Gordon M. Redding - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):77-78.
  • Animal-environment mutuality and direct perception.Sandra S. Prindle, Claudia Carello & M. T. Turvey - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):395-397.
  • How wrong is Gibson?K. Prazdny - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):394-395.
  • Mathematics and the Good Life.Stephen Pollard - 2013 - Philosophia Mathematica 21 (1):93-109.
    We mathematical animals should be grateful that mathematics is instrumentally useful. We should not, however, forget its other contributions to human happiness. Bertrand Russell and John Dewey offer timely reminders that provide insight into the role of non-mathematicians in the evaluation of mathematics.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A trans-actional approach to moral development.Matthew Pamental - 2010 - Ethics and Education 5 (1):15-26.
    Among the latest trends in moral educational theory, several authors have suggested that a sociocultural approach to moral education is an improvement over the dominant cognitive-developmental and character educational paradigms. This approach draws its inspiration from the work of the Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky. In the 1920s, Vygotsky attempted to reconstruct psychology to overcome the false dichotomy psychologists had posited between the individual and the environment. This genre of sociocultural theory has come to be known as activity theory. Despite its (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Position information versus motor programs: two levels of sensorimotor theory.Kenneth R. Paap - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):77-77.
  • Learning as Becoming Conscious: A note on Jablonka and Ginsburg’s Notion of Learning.Alin Olteanu - 2022 - Biosemiotics 15 (3):457-467.
    This commentary addresses the concept of learning stemming from Eva Jablonka and Simona Ginsburg’s theory of the emergence of consciousness. Jablonka and Ginsburg find strong support in biosemiotics for their argument that learning offers an evolutionary transition marker for the emergence of consciousness. Indeed, biosemiotics embraces a view on evolution that integrates both phylogeny and ontogeny. It does not polarize learning and evolving. At the same time, Jablonka and Ginsburg’s argument gives both biosemiotics and learning theory a shake, forcing scholarship (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Environmental Crisis as a Good Case for an Intellectual and Practical Integration Between Philosophy and Science.Nei de Freitas Nunes-Neto - 2015 - Science & Education 24 (9-10):1285-1299.
  • Foldor' solipsisms: dont's look a gift horse in the ….Donald A. Norman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):90-90.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Centrifugal contributions to visual perceptual after effects.K. S. K. Murthy - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):77-77.
  • Visual-motor conflict resolved by motor adaptation without perceptual change.Joel M. Miller - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):76-76.
  • Adaptation of the distortion of shape is different from adaptation to the distortion of space.H. H. Mikaelian - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):76-76.
  • Constructivist Set-Theoretic Analysis: An Alternative to Essentialist Social Science.James Mahoney - 2023 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 53 (4):327-366.
    Psychological essentialism is a cognitive bias through which human beings conceive the entities around them as having inner essences and basic natures. Social scientists routinely generate flawed inferences because their methods require the truth of psychological essentialism. This article develops set-theoretic analysis as a scientific-constructivist approach that overcomes the bias of psychological essentialism. With this approach, the “sets” of set-theoretic analysis are mental phenomena that establish boundaries and identify similarities and differences among entities whose natural kind composition is not known. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Perceptual activity and direct perception.William M. Mace - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):392-393.
  • Non-Visual Determinants of Perception.Arien Mack - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):75-76.
  • Are mediating representations the ghosts in the machine?Alan K. Mackworth - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):393-394.
  • Visual perception: the shifting domain of discourse.Geoffrey R. Loftus & Elizabeth F. Loftus - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):391-392.
  • Syntax, functional semantics, and referential semantics.Brian F. Loar - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):89-90.
  • The Other Social Science: Three centuries of common heterodoxy.Peter Lenco - 2023 - Thesis Eleven 175 (1):3-26.
    This paper starts with the observation that at least for the last century there has been an orthodoxy in the social sciences characterized by sui generis structures of various kinds but also (paradoxically) by the unique role of individuals in their ability to intervene in the flow of events. This paper argues that there is a commonality to a number of challenges to orthodoxy that dates back to the beginnings of the social sciences themselves with Vico. Although many connections have (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The encoding of spatial position in the brain.Joseph S. Lappin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):74-75.
  • Complexity and Relations.Jeanette Elizabeth Lancaster - 2013 - Educational Philosophy and Theory 45 (12):1264-1275.
    A central feature of complexity is that it is based on non-linear, recursive relations. However, in most current accounts of complexity such relations, while non-linear, are based on the reductive relations of a Newtonian onto-epistemological framework. This means that the systems that are emergent from the workings of such relations are a narrowly reduced spectrum of complex systems. It is argued that John Dewey’s trans-actional relations, relations that are characterized by an irreducible internal distinction, can function as an exemplar of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Is the “cognitive penetrability” criterion invalidated by contemporary physics?Peter N. Kugler, M. T. Turvey & Robert Shaw - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):303-306.
  • A provisional sensory/motor “complementarity” model for adaptation effects.Ivo Kohler - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):73-74.
  • Why argue about direct perception?J. J. Koenderink - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):390-391.
  • The information inelasticity of habits: Kahneman’s bounded rationality or Simon’s procedural rationality?Elias L. Khalil - 2022 - Synthese 200 (4):1-40.
    Why would decision makers adopt heuristics, priors, or in short “habits” that prevent them from optimally using pertinent information—even when such information is freely-available? One answer, Herbert Simon’s “procedural rationality” regards the question invalid: DMs do not, and in fact cannot, process information in an optimal fashion. For Simon, habits are the primitives, where humans are ready to replace them only when they no longer sustain a pregiven “satisficing” goal. An alternative answer, Daniel Kahneman’s “mental economy” regards the question valid: (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Direct vs. representational views of cognition: A parallel between vision and phonology.Samuel Jay Keyser & Steven Pinker - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):389-390.
  • Motor-sensory feedback formulations: are we asking the right questions?J. A. Scott Kelso - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):72-73.
  • Fodor's guide to cognitive psychology.Jerrold J. Katz - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):85-89.
  • Implications of Fodor' methodological solipsism for psychological theories.Peter W. Jusczyk & Bruce Earhard - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):84-85.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The content of a representation also depends on the procedure interpreting it.A. K. Joshi - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):84-84.
  • On the nature of information in behalf of direct perception.Rebecca K. Jones & Anne D. Pick - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):388-389.
  • Direct perception and perceptual processes.Gunnar Johansson, Claes von Hofsten & Gunnar Jansson - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):388-388.
  • Visuomotor experiments: Failure to replicate, or failure to match the theory?Marc Jeannerod - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):71-71.
  • Philosophy of Education in a New Key: Who Remembers Greta Thunberg? Education and Environment after the Coronavirus.Petar Jandrić, Jimmy Jaldemark, Zoe Hurley, Brendan Bartram, Adam Matthews, Michael Jopling, Julia Mañero, Alison MacKenzie, Jones Irwin, Ninette Rothmüller, Benjamin Green, Shane J. Ralston, Olli Pyyhtinen, Sarah Hayes, Jake Wright, Michael A. Peters & Marek Tesar - 2021 - Educational Philosophy and Theory 53 (14):1421-1441.
    This paper explores relationships between environment and education after the Covid-19 pandemic through the lens of philosophy of education in a new key developed by Michael Peters and the Philosophy of Education Society of Australasia. The paper is collectively written by 15 authors who responded to the question: Who remembers Greta Thunberg? Their answers are classified into four main themes and corresponding sections. The first section, ‘As we bake the earth, let's try and bake it from scratch’, gathers wider philosophical (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Sensing and reference.S. D. Isard - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):83-84.
  • Material Engagement Theory and its philosophical ties to pragmatism.Antonis Iliopoulos - 2019 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 18 (1):39-63.
    Material Engagement Theory is currently driving a conceptual change in the archaeology of mind. Drawing upon the dictates of enactivism and active externalism, it specifically calls for a radical reconceptualization of mind and material culture. Unpersuaded by the common assumption that cognition is brain-bound, Malafouris argues in favour of a process ontology that situates thinking in action. In granting ontological primacy to material engagement, MET seeks to illuminate the emergence of human ways of thinking through the practical effects of the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Paul Goodman redux: education as apprenticed anarchism.M. Andrew Holowchak - 2010 - Ethics and Education 5 (3):217 - 232.
    When talk of philosophy of pedagogy comes up today, it is common to hear the names of Aristotle, Thomas Jefferson, John Dewey, or Paulo Freire, but the name of Paul Goodman, who campaigned vigorously for pedagogical reform much of his life, is seldom mentioned. In spite of neglect of his work, Goodman had much to say on pedagogical practice that is rich, poignant, and relevant today. In consequence, it is unfortunate that he is seldom read and discussed today. This essay (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Is there curvature adaptation not attributable to purely intravisual phenomena?Julian Hochberg & Leon Festinger - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):71-71.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inferring the meaning of direct perception.Geoffrey E. Hinton - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):387-388.
  • Can the brain be divided into a sensory and a motor part?Volker Henn - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):70-71.
  • Mediating the so-called immediate processes of perception.Frederick Hayes-Roth - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):386-387.
  • Knowing about formality.Pat Hayes - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):82-83.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Formality and naturalism.John Haugeland - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):81-82.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation