Results for 'FIXATION ERROR, PERIPHERAL STIMULI'

994 found
Order:
  1.  13
    Fixation errors in eye movements to peripheral stimuli.Albert E. Bartz - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (4):444.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  32
    Effect of incentives upon reactions to peripheral stimuli.Harry P. Bahrick, Paul M. Fitts & Robert E. Rankin - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 44 (6):400.
  3.  17
    Peripherally presented and unreported words may bias the perceived meaning of a centrally fixated homograph.John L. Bradshaw - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (6):1200.
  4.  29
    On peripheral and central processes in vision: Inferences from an information-processing analysis of masking with patterned stimuli.M. T. Turvey - 1973 - Psychological Review 80 (1):1-52.
  5.  8
    Visual Fixation Patterns During Viewing of Half-Face Stimuli in Adults: An Eye-Tracking Study.Ágoston Galambos, Borbála Turcsán, Katalin Oláh, Fruzsina Elekes, Anna Gergely, Ildikó Király & József Topál - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  11
    Time-errors in the method of single stimuli.C. C. Pratt - 1933 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 16 (6):798.
  7.  43
    Attentional inhibition mediates inattentional blindness.Preston P. Thakral & Scott D. Slotnick - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (2):636-643.
    Salient stimuli presented at unattended locations are not always perceived, a phenomenon termed inattentional blindness. We hypothesized that inattentional blindness may be mediated by attentional inhibition. It has been shown that attentional inhibition effects are maximal near an attended location. If our hypothesis is correct, inattentional blindness effects should similarly be maximal near an attended location. During central fixation, participants viewed rapidly presented colored digits at a peripheral location. An unexpected black circle was concurrently presented. Participants were (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  8.  9
    Studies from the Bryn Mawr college Laboratory. The effect of the brightness of background on the appearance of color stimuli in peripheral vision.Grace Maxwell Fernald - 1908 - Psychological Review 15 (1):25-43.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  26
    Variability of attention bias in socially anxious adolescents: differences in fixation duration toward adult and adolescent face stimuli.Andrea Trubanova Wieckowski, Nicole N. Capriola-Hall, Rebecca Elias, Thomas H. Ollendick & Susan W. White - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (4):825-831.
    ABSTRACTPrior research on attention bias in anxious youth, often utilising a visual dot probe task, has yielded inconsistent findings, which may be due to how bias is assessed and/or variability in the phenomenon. The present study utilises eye gaze tracking to assess attention bias in socially anxious adolescents, and explores several methodological and within-subject factors that may contribute to variability in attention bias. Attention bias to threat was measured in forty-two treatment-seeking adolescents diagnosed with Social Anxiety Disorder. Bias scores toward (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10.  7
    A novel variation of the Stroop task reveals reflexive supremacy of peripheral over gaze stimuli in pro and anti saccades.Liran Zeligman & Ari Z. Zivotofsky - 2020 - Consciousness and Cognition 85:103020.
  11.  18
    Experimental test of the relation of confidence with the time errors in the method of constant stimuli.Sergio Cesare Masin & Gianpaolo Ceretta - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (1):31-34.
  12.  7
    Parallel Adaptation to Spatially Distinct Distortions.Yannick Sauer, Siegfried Wahl & Katharina Rifai - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Optical distortions as a visual disturbance are inherent in many optical devices such as spectacles or virtual reality headsets. In such devices, distortions vary spatially across the visual field. In progressive addition lenses, for example, the left and right regions of the lens skew the peripheral parts of the wearers visual field in opposing directions. The human visual system adapts to homogeneous distortions and the respective aftereffects are transferred to non-retinotopic locations. This study investigates simultaneous adaptation to two opposing (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  10
    Numerosity Perception in Peripheral Vision.Min Susan Li, Clement Abbatecola, Lucy S. Petro & Lars Muckli - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Peripheral vision has different functional priorities for mammals than foveal vision. One of its roles is to monitor the environment while central vision is focused on the current task. Becoming distracted too easily would be counterproductive in this perspective, so the brain should react to behaviourally relevant changes. Gist processing is good for this purpose, and it is therefore not surprising that evidence from both functional brain imaging and behavioural research suggests a tendency to generalize and blend information in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  11
    Some sources of error in half-heaviness judgments.Trygg Engen & Ülker Tulunay - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 54 (3):208.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  15.  53
    Investigating Arousal, Saccade Preparation, and Global Luminance Effects on Microsaccade Behavior.Jui-Tai Chen, Rachel Yep, Yu-Fan Hsu, Yih-Giun Cherng & Chin-An Wang - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Microsaccades, small saccadic eye movements occurring during fixation, have been suggested to be modulated by various sensory, cognitive, and affective processes relating to arousal. Although the modulation of fatigue-related arousal on microsaccade behavior has previously been characterized, the influence of other aspects of arousal, such as emotional arousal, is less understood. Moreover, microsaccades are modulated by cognitive processes that could also be linked to arousal. To investigate the influence of emotional arousal, saccade preparation, and global luminance levels on microsaccade (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  28
    Serial-list items as stimuli in paired-associate learning.Sheldon M. Ebenholtz - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (1):154.
    Previous experiments have shown a serial-position effect (SPE) in paired-associate (PA) learning where the pairs contained stimuli pre- viously learned in serial order. The present experiment extended the number of pairs from 10 to 14. Pairs containing stimuli from terminal serial positions were learned with significantly fewer errors than pairs whose stimuli derived from central positions. The latter produced a dip in the PA error distribution suggesting the presence of sequential associations in SL between items occupying central (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  34
    We believe the error theory.John Alton Christmann - 2022 - Metaphilosophy 53 (5):632-644.
    Bart Streumer thinks that we cannot believe the global normative error theory. Streumer's argument presupposes a Cartesian theory of belief fixation. The Cartesian theory entails that we can understand a proposition without believing it. But the Cartesian theory of belief fixation is false, and the Spinozan theory is true. The Spinozan theory of belief fixation entails that we cannot understand a proposition without believing it. The present paper argues that Streumer's claim is false, and we can believe (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  31
    Language-guided visual processing affects reasoning: The role of referential and spatial anchoring.Magda L. Dumitru, Gitte H. Joergensen, Alice G. Cruickshank & Gerry T. M. Altmann - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (2):562-571.
    Language is more than a source of information for accessing higher-order conceptual knowledge. Indeed, language may determine how people perceive and interpret visual stimuli. Visual processing in linguistic contexts, for instance, mirrors language processing and happens incrementally, rather than through variously-oriented fixations over a particular scene. The consequences of this atypical visual processing are yet to be determined. Here, we investigated the integration of visual and linguistic input during a reasoning task. Participants listened to sentences containing conjunctions or disjunctions (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  19.  36
    The neural substrates associated with inattentional blindness.Preston P. Thakral - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1768-1775.
    Inattentional blindness is the failure to perceive salient stimuli presented at unattended locations. Whereas the behavioral manifestation of inattentional blindness has been investigated, the neural basis of this phenomenon has remained elusive. In the current study, event-related fMRI was used to identify the neural substrates associated with inattentional blindness. During central fixation, participants named colored digits presented at a peripheral location. On a subset of trials, an unexpected checkerboard circle was presented at the same eccentricity along with (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20.  10
    Time errors in the discrimination of color mass by the ranking method.B. R. Philip - 1940 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 27 (3):285.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  32
    Two attentional components for two purposes.B. Fischer & H. Weber - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):770-771.
    Inappropriate saccades are prevented by fixation and by voluntary attention. The fixation system inhibits the saccade system. Like monkeys without a fixation system, humans with a weak fixation system produce many express saccades and cannot suppress prosaccades in an antisaccade task. With permanent attention to a peripheral location only a few express saccades to a stimulus at this location can be elicited: the sustained component of attention acts like fixation. When attention is captured by (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  18
    Modeling Lag‐2 Revisits to Understand Trade‐Offs in Mixed Control of Fixation Termination During Visual Search.J. Godwin Hayward, D. Reichle Erik & Menneer Tamaryn - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (4):996-1019.
    An important question about eye-movement behavior is when the decision is made to terminate a fixation and program the following saccade. Different approaches have found converging evidence in favor of a mixed-control account, in which there is some overlap between processing information at fixation and planning the following saccade. We examined one interesting instance of mixed control in visual search: lag-2 revisits, during which observers fixate a stimulus, move to a different stimulus, and then revisit the first stimulus (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  23.  43
    Attentional bias toward low-intensity stimuli: An explanation for the intensity dissociation between reaction time and temporal order judgment?Piotr Jaskowski & Rolf Verleger - 2000 - Consciousness and Cognition 9 (3):435-456.
    If two stimuli need different times to be processed, this difference should in principle be reflected both by response times (RT) and by judgments of their temporal order (TOJ). However, several dissociations have been reported between RT and TOJ, e.g., RT is more affected than TOJ when stimulus intensity decreases. One account for these dissociations is to assume differences in the allocation of attention induced by the two tasks. To test this hypothesis, different distributions of attention were induced in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  24.  11
    Inborn errors of GABA metabolism.Kenneth M. Gibson, William L. Nyhan & Jaak Jaeken - 1986 - Bioessays 4 (1):24-27.
    Defects in man in four steps of 4‐aminobutyric acid (GABA) metabolism may interefere with the function of this major inhibitory neurotransmitter. Glutamic acid decarboxylase, 4‐aminobutyric acid aminotransferase, succinic semialdehyde dehydrogenase, and homocarnosinase are closely identified with the brain, but two of these enzymes are expressed in cultured peripheral cells, which may permit novel approaches to the study of the metabolism and regulation of GABA.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25. The impact of error-consequence severity on cue processing in importance-biased prospective memory.Kristina Krasich, Eva Gjorgieva, Samuel Murray, Shreya Bhatia, Myrthe Faber, Felipe De Brigard & Marty Woldorff - forthcoming - Cerebral Cortex Communications.
    Prospective memory (PM) enables people to remember to complete important tasks in the future. Failing to do so can result in consequences of varying severity. Here, we investigated how PM error-consequence severity impacts the neural processing of relevant cues for triggering PM and the ramification of that processing on the associated prospective task performance. Participants role-played a cafeteria worker serving lunches to fictitious students and had to remember to deliver an alternative lunch to students (as PM cues) who would otherwise (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  15
    Time-order errors in comparative judgments of hurtfulness.Robert H. Geertsma - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (3):284.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  14
    Time order error in successive judgments and in reflexes. I. Inhibition of the judgment and the reflex.H. Peak - 1939 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 25 (6):535.
  28.  10
    Impact of Distracting Emotional Stimuli on the Characteristics of Movement Performance: A Kinematic Study.Yingzhi Lu, Tianyi Wang, Qiuping Long & Zijian Cheng - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    It is well-documented that emotional stimuli impact both the cognitive and motor aspects of “goal-directed” behavior. However, how emotional distractors impact motor performance remains unclear. This study aimed to characterize how movement quality was impacted during emotional distractors. We used a modified oddball paradigm and documented the performance of pure movement. Participants were designated to draw a triangle or a polygon, while an emotional stimulus was presented. Speed was assessed using reaction time and movement time. The quality and precision (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  50
    Are there fundamental differences in the peripheral mechanisms of visceral and somatic pain?Stephen B. McMahon - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (3):381-391.
    There are some conspicuous differences between the sensibilities of cutaneous and visceral tissues: (1) Direct trauma, which readily produces pain when applied to the skin, is mostly without effect in healthy visceral tissue. (2) Pain that arises from visceral tissues is initially often poorly localised and diffuse. (3) With time, visceral pains are often referred to more superficial structures. (4) The site of referred pain may also show hyperalgesia. (5) In disease states, the afflicted viscera may also become hyperalgesic. In (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  9
    The time order error in successive judgments and in reflexes: II. As a function of the first stimulus of a pair.H. Peak - 1940 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 26 (1):103.
  31.  25
    The processing of auditory and visual recognition of self-stimuli.Susan M. Hughes & Shevon E. Nicholson - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (4):1124-1134.
    This study examined self-recognition processing in both the auditory and visual modalities by determining how comparable hearing a recording of one’s own voice was to seeing photograph of one’s own face. We also investigated whether the simultaneous presentation of auditory and visual self-stimuli would either facilitate or inhibit self-identification. Ninety-one participants completed reaction-time tasks of self-recognition when presented with their own faces, own voices, and combinations of the two. Reaction time and errors made when responding with both the right (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  32.  63
    Modularity in developmental disorders: Evidence from specific language impairment and peripheral dyslexias.Naama Friedmann & Aviah Gvion - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (6):756-757.
    Evidence from various subtypes of Specific Language Impairment and developmental peripheral dyslexias is presented to support the idea that even developmental disorders can be modular. However, in developmental letter position dyslexia and neglect dyslexia we show that additional errors can occur because of insufficient orthographic-lexical knowledge.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  33.  19
    People look at the object they fear: oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal threat.Tom Nissens, Michel Failing & Jan Theeuwes - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (8):1707-1714.
    ABSTRACTIt is known that people covertly attend to threatening stimuli even when it is not beneficial for the task. In the current study we examined whether overt selection is affected by the presence of an object that signals threat. We demonstrate that stimuli that signal the possibility of receiving an electric shock capture the eyes more often than stimuli signalling no shock. Capture occurred even though the threat-signalling stimulus was neither physically salient nor task relevant at any (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  34.  8
    The Differential Effects of Auditory and Visual Stimuli on Learning, Retention and Reactivation of a Perceptual-Motor Temporal Sequence in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder.Mélody Blais, Mélanie Jucla, Stéphanie Maziero, Jean-Michel Albaret, Yves Chaix & Jessica Tallet - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    This study investigates the procedural learning, retention, and reactivation of temporal sensorimotor sequences in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Twenty typically-developing children and 12 children with DCD took part in this study. The children were required to tap on a keyboard, synchronizing with auditory or visual stimuli presented as an isochronous temporal sequence, and practice non-isochronous temporal sequences to memorize them. Immediate and delayed retention of the audio-motor and visuo-motor non-isochronous sequences were tested by removing auditory or (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  35.  15
    “Economies of Experience”-Disambiguation of Degraded Stimuli Leads to a Decreased Dispersion of Eye-Movement Patterns.Magdalena Ewa Król & Michał Król - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S3):728-756.
    We demonstrate “economies of experience” in eye-movement patterns—that is, optimization of eye-movement patterns aimed at more efficient and less costly visual processing, similar to the priming-induced formation of sparser cortical representations or reduced reaction times. Participants looked at Mooney-type, degraded stimuli that were difficult to recognize without prior experience, but easily recognizable after exposure to their undegraded versions. As predicted, eye-movement dispersion, velocity, and the number of fixations decreased with each stimulus presentation. Further analyses showed that this effect was (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36.  43
    Fearing shades of grey: individual differences in fear responding towards generalisation stimuli.Inna Arnaudova, Angelos-Miltiadis Krypotos, Marieke Effting, Merel Kindt & Tom Beckers - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (6):1181-1196.
    ABSTRACTIndividual differences in fear generalisation have been proposed to play a role in the aetiology and/or maintenance of anxiety disorders, but few data are available to directly support that claim. The research that is available has focused mostly on generalisation of peripheral and central physiological fear responses. Far less is known about the generalisation of avoidance, the behavioural component of fear. In two experiments, we evaluated how neuroticism, a known vulnerability factor for anxiety, modulates an array of fear responses, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  4
    Do gaze and non-gaze stimuli trigger different spatial interference effects? It depends on stimulus perceivability.Zhe Chen, Rebecca H. Thomas & Makayla S. Chen - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Among the studies on the perception of gaze vs. non-gaze stimuli, some have shown that the two types of stimuli trigger different patterns of attentional effects, while others have reported no such differences. In three experiments, we investigated the role of stimulus perceivability in spatial interference effects when the targets were gaze vs. non-gaze stimuli. We used a spatial Stroop task that required participants to make a speeded response to the direction indicated by the targets located on (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  9
    Magnetoencephalography Responses to Unpredictable and Predictable Rare Somatosensory Stimuli in Healthy Adult Humans.Qianru Xu, Chaoxiong Ye, Jarmo A. Hämäläinen, Elisa M. Ruohonen, Xueqiao Li & Piia Astikainen - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Mismatch brain responses to unpredicted rare stimuli are suggested to be a neural indicator of prediction error, but this has rarely been studied in the somatosensory modality. Here, we investigated how the brain responds to unpredictable and predictable rare events. Magnetoencephalography responses were measured in adults frequently presented with somatosensory stimuli that were occasionally replaced by two consecutively presented rare stimuli [unpredictable rare stimulus and predictable rare stimulus ; p = 0.1 for each]. The FRE and PR (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  11
    Verbal paired-associate learning as a function of grouping similar stimuli or responses.Iris C. Rotberg & Myron Woolman - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (1):47.
  40.  23
    Considering the Prevalence of the "Stimulus Error" in Color Naming Research.Kimberly Jameson, Debi Roberson, Don Dedrick & David Bimler - 2007 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (1-2):119-142.
    In "Does the Basic Color Terms discussion suffer from the Stimulus Error?" Rolf Kuehni describes a research stumbling block known as the "stimulus error," and hints at the difficulties it causes for mainstream color naming research. Among the issues intrinsic to Kuehni's "stimulus error" description is the important question of what can generally be inferred from color naming behaviors based on bounded samples of empirical stimuli. Here we examine some specifics of the color naming research issues that Kuehni raises. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  13
    Shape discrimination as a function of the angular orientation of the stimuli.Malcolm D. Arnoult - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (5):323.
  42.  14
    Two-point discrimination in visual space as a function of the temporal interval between the stimuli.Michael Leyzorek - 1951 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 41 (5):364.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43. Contemplative Practices: The Cultivation of Discernment in Mind and Heart,”.Cognitive Error - 2009 - Buddhist-Christian Studies 29:59-79.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44. E. Narmous, The Analysis and Cognition of Melodic Complexity. Chicago.B. J. Baars, Human Error New, R. A. Finke, V. A. Bradley, N. J. Hillsdale, Leab de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. MacNeilage & J. Morton - 1994 - Cognition 52:159-162.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. Predictive brains, dreaming selves, sleeping bodies: how the analysis of dream movement can inform a theory of self- and world-simulation in dreams.Jennifer M. Windt - 2018 - Synthese 195 (6):2577-2625.
    In this paper, I discuss the relationship between bodily experiences in dreams and the sleeping, physical body. I question the popular view that dreaming is a naturally and frequently occurring real-world example of cranial envatment. This view states that dreams are functionally disembodied states: in a majority of dreams, phenomenal experience, including the phenomenology of embodied selfhood, unfolds completely independently of external and peripheral stimuli and outward movement. I advance an alternative and more empirically plausible view of dreams (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  46. The timing of conscious experience: A critical review and reinterpretation of Libet's research.Gilberto Gomes - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (4):559-595.
    An extended examination of Libet's works led to a comprehensive reinterpretation of his results. According to this reinterpretation, the Minimum Train Duration of electrical brain stimulation should be considered as the time needed to create a brain stimulus efficient for producing conscious sensation and not as a basis for inferring the latency for conscious sensation of peripheral origin. Latency for conscious sensation with brain stimulation may occurafterthe Minimum Train Duration. Backward masking with cortical stimuli suggests a 125-300 ms (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  47.  75
    Studying Spatial Visual Attention: The Attention-Window Task as a Measurement Tool for the Shape and Maximum Spread of the Attention Window.Stefanie Klatt & Daniel Memmert - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Visual attentional processes have been an important topic in psychological research for years. Over the last few decades, new methods have been developed, aiming to explore the characteristics of the focus of attention in more detail. Studies that applied the “Attention-Window Task” quantified the maximum extent of the “Attention Window” along its horizontal, vertical, and diagonal meridians, when subjects were required to perceive two peripheral stimuli simultaneously. In three experiments using the AWT, we investigated the effects of cue (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  6
    Using Blur for Perceptual Investigation and Training in Sport? A Clear Picture of the Evidence and Implications for Future Research.Annabelle Limballe, Richard Kulpa & Simon Bennett - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Dynamic, interactive sports require athletes to identify, pick-up and process relevant information in a very limited time, in order to then make an appropriate response. Perceptual-cognitive skills are, therefore, a key determinant of elite sporting performance. Recently, sport scientists have investigated ways to assess and train perceptual-cognitive skills, with one such method involving the use of blurred stimuli. Here, we describe the two main methods used to generate blur and then review the current findings in a sports context. Overall, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  49.  51
    Neuroethology of releasing mechanisms: Prey-catching in toads.Jörg-Peter Ewert - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):337-368.
    Abstract“Sign stimuli” elicit specific patterns of behavior when an organism's motivation is appropriate. In the toad, visually released prey-catching involves orienting toward the prey, approaching, fixating, and snapping. For these action patterns to be selected and released, the prey must be recognized and localized in space. Toads discriminate prey from nonprey by certain spatiotemporal stimulus features. The stimulus-response relations are mediated by innate releasing mechanisms (RMs) with recognition properties partly modifiable by experience. Striato-pretecto-tectal connectivity determines the RM's recognition and (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   97 citations  
  50. Predictive Processing and Object Recognition.Berit Brogaard & Thomas Alrik Sørensen - 2024 - In Tony Cheng, Ryoji Sato & Jakob Hohwy (eds.), Expected Experiences: The Predictive Mind in an Uncertain World. Routledge. pp. 112–139.
    Predictive processing models of perception take issue with standard models of perception as hierarchical bottom-up processing modulated by memory and attention. The predictive framework posits that the brain generates predictions about stimuli, which are matched to the incoming signal. Mismatches between predictions and the incoming signal – so-called prediction errors – are then used to generate new and better predictions until the prediction errors have been minimized, at which point a perception arises. Predictive models hold that all bottom-up processes (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 994