Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Interfield theories.Lindley Darden & Nancy Maull - 1977 - Philosophy of Science 44 (1):43-64.
    This paper analyzes the generation and function of hitherto ignored or misrepresented interfield theories , theories which bridge two fields of science. Interfield theories are likely to be generated when two fields share an interest in explaining different aspects of the same phenomenon and when background knowledge already exists relating the two fields. The interfield theory functions to provide a solution to a characteristic type of theoretical problem: how are the relations between fields to be explained? In solving this problem (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   266 citations  
  • The Modularity of Mind.Robert Cummins & Jerry Fodor - 1983 - Philosophical Review 94 (1):101.
  • A Study of Thinking.Jerome S. Bruner, Jacqueline J. Goodnow & George A. Austin - 1958 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 19 (1):118-119.
  • Natural deduction in connectionist systems.William Bechtel - 1994 - Synthese 101 (3):433-463.
    The relation between logic and thought has long been controversial, but has recently influenced theorizing about the nature of mental processes in cognitive science. One prominent tradition argues that to explain the systematicity of thought we must posit syntactically structured representations inside the cognitive system which can be operated upon by structure sensitive rules similar to those employed in systems of natural deduction. I have argued elsewhere that the systematicity of human thought might better be explained as resulting from the (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • The role of theories in conceptual coherence.G. L. Murphy & D. L. Medin - 1999 - In Eric Margolis & Stephen Laurence (eds.), Concepts: Core Readings. MIT Press. pp. 289--316.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   276 citations  
  • A Mathematical Theory of Communication.Claude Elwood Shannon - 1948 - Bell System Technical Journal 27 (April 1924):379–423.
    The mathematical theory of communication.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1179 citations  
  • Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again.Tim van Gelder & Andy Clark - 1998 - Philosophical Review 107 (4):647.
    A great deal of philosophy of mind in the modern era has been driven by an intense aversion to Cartesian dualism. In the 1950s, materialists claimed to have succeeded once and for all in exorcising the Cartesian ghost by identifying the mind with the brain. In subsequent decades, cognitive science put scientific meat on this metaphysical skeleton by explicating mental processes as digital computation implemented in the brain's hardware.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   339 citations  
  • The role of theories in conceptual coherence.Gregory L. Murphy & Douglas L. Medin - 1985 - Psychological Review 92 (3):289-316.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   486 citations  
  • Context theory of classification learning.Douglas L. Medin & Marguerite M. Schaffer - 1978 - Psychological Review 85 (3):207-238.
  • Review of Larry May, Marilyn Friedman and Andy Clark: Mind and Morals: Essays on Ethics and Cognitive Science.[REVIEW]G. F. Schueler - 1997 - Ethics 107 (2):349-351.
  • The Language of Thought.Charles E. Marks - 1978 - Philosophical Review 87 (1):108.
  • Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind.Dan Lloyd - 1991 - Philosophical Review 100 (2):289.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   76 citations  
  • A unified account of cognitive impairments following frontal lobe damage: the role of working memory in complex, organized behavior.Daniel Y. Kimberg & Martha J. Farah - 1993 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 122 (4):411.
  • Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis.Jerry A. Fodor & Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1988 - Cognition 28 (1-2):3-71.
    This paper explores the difference between Connectionist proposals for cognitive a r c h i t e c t u r e a n d t h e s o r t s o f m o d e l s t hat have traditionally been assum e d i n c o g n i t i v e s c i e n c e . W e c l a i m t h a t t h (...)
    Direct download (13 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1119 citations  
  • The Language of Thought.J. A. Fodor - 1978 - Critica 10 (28):140-143.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1388 citations  
  • Mind As Motion.T. van Gelder & Robert Port (eds.) - 1995 - MIT Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   84 citations  
  • Components of conceptual representation. From feature lists to recursive frames.Lawrence Barsalou & Christopher Hale - 1993 - In I. Van Mechelen, J. Hampton, R. Michalski & P. Theuns (eds.), Categories and Concepts: Theoretical Views and Inductive Data Analysis. Academic Press. pp. 97-144.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Psychosemantics, the Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind. [REVIEW]Jan Wolenski - 1991 - Studia Logica 50 (2):356-357.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   129 citations  
  • Imagistic representation.Jerry A. Fodor - 1980 - In Ned Block (ed.), Readings in Philosophy of Psychology, Volume II. Harvard University Press. pp. 135-149.
  • Cognition in the Wild.Edward Hutchins - 1995 - Critica 27 (81):101-105.
  • Explorations in the dynamics of cognition.T. Van Gelder & R. F. Port - 1995 - In T. Van Gelder & Robert Port (eds.), Mind as Motion. MIT Press.
  • Fast thinking.Daniel C. Dennett - 1987 - In The Intentional Stance. MIT Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • Multiple memory systems and consciousness.Endel Tulving - 1987 - Human Neurobiology 6:67-80.