Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Empirical treatments of imagination and creativity.Dustin Stokes - 2024 - In Amy Kind & Julia Langkau (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Imagination and Creativity. Oxford University Press.
    This paper offers a critical survey and analysis of empirical studies on creativity, with emphasis on how imagination plays a role in the creative process. It takes as a foil the romantic view that, given features like novelty, incubation, and insight, we should be skeptical about the prospects for naturalistic explanation of creativity. It rebuts this skepticism by first distinguishing stages or operations in the creative process. It then works through various behavioral and neural studies, and corresponding philosophical theorizing, that (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Naturalistic Approaches to Creativity.Dustin Stokes & Elliot Samuel Paul - 2016 - In Justin Sytsma & Wesley Buckwalter (eds.), A Companion to Experimental Philosophy. Malden, MA: Wiley. pp. 318–333.
    This chapter offers a brief characterization of creativity, followed by a review of some of the reasons people have been skeptical about the possibility of explaining creativity. It surveys some of the recent work on creativity that is naturalistic in the sense that it presumes creativity is natural, as opposed to magical, occult, or supernatural, and is therefore amenable to scientific inquiry. The chapter divides into two categories. The broader category is empirical philosophy, which draws on empirical research while addressing (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Imagination and Creativity.Dustin Stokes - 2016 - In Amy Kind (ed.), The Routledge Handbook of the Philosophy of Imagination. New York: Routledge.
    This paper surveys historical and recent philosophical discussions of the relations between imagination and creativity. In the first two sections, it covers two insufficiently studied analyses of the creative imagination, that of Kant and Sartre, respectively. The next section discusses imagination and its role in scientific discovery, with particular emphasis on the writings of Michael Polanyi, and on thought experiments and experimental design. The final section offers a brief discussion of some very recent work done on conceptual relations between imagination (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Origins of the Qualitative Aspects of Consciousness: Evolutionary Answers to Chalmers' Hard Problem.Jonathan Y. Tsou - 2012 - In Liz Swan (ed.), Origins of Mind. Springer. pp. 259--269.
    According to David Chalmers, the hard problem of consciousness consists of explaining how and why qualitative experience arises from physical states. Moreover, Chalmers argues that materialist and reductive explanations of mentality are incapable of addressing the hard problem. In this chapter, I suggest that Chalmers’ hard problem can be usefully distinguished into a ‘how question’ and ‘why question,’ and I argue that evolutionary biology has the resources to address the question of why qualitative experience arises from brain states. From this (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Machine discoverers: Transforming the spaces they explore.Jan M. Zytkow - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):557-558.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The time course of implicit and explicit concept learning.Eleni Ziori & Zoltán Dienes - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (1):204-216.
    The present experiment investigated the development of implicit and explicit knowledge during concept learning. According to Cleeremans and Jiménez , the content of a representation can be conscious only when the representation is of a sufficiently good quality; on this theory, increasing explicit and decreasing implicit knowledge might be expected with training. The view that implicit knowledge arises from compilation of explicit knowledge makes the opposite prediction. The present research tested these possibilities using subjective measures based on confidence ratings. One (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Analogy Lays the Foundation for Two Crucial Aspects of Symbolic Development: Intention and Correspondence.Lei Yuan & David H. Uttal - 2017 - Topics in Cognitive Science 9 (3):738-757.
    We argue that analogical reasoning, particularly Gentner's structure-mapping theory, provides an integrative theoretical framework through which we can better understand the development of symbol use. Analogical reasoning can contribute both to the understanding of others’ intentions and the establishment of correspondences between symbols and their referents, two crucial components of symbolic understanding. We review relevant research on the development of symbolic representations, intentionality, comparison, and similarity, and demonstrate how structure-mapping theory can shed light on several ostensibly disparate findings in the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The creative mind versus the creative computer.Robert W. Weisberg - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):555-557.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The empirical detection of creativity.Han L. J. van der Maas & Peter C. M. Molenaar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):555-555.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Children's Mental Models of Prenatal Development.Tessa J. P. van Schijndel, Sara E. van Es, Rooske K. Franse, Bianca M. C. W. van Bers & Maartje E. J. Raijmakers - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Visualizing Thought.Barbara Tversky - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (3):499-535.
    Depictive expressions of thought predate written language by thousands of years. They have evolved in communities through a kind of informal user testing that has refined them. Analyzing common visual communications reveals consistencies that illuminate how people think as well as guide design; the process can be brought into the laboratory and accelerated. Like language, visual communications abstract and schematize; unlike language, they use properties of the page (e.g., proximity and place: center, horizontal/up–down, vertical/left–right) and the marks on it (e.g., (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • The creative mind.Scott R. Turner - 1995 - Artificial Intelligence 79 (1):145-159.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Creativity: Myths? Mechanisms.Michel Treisman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):554-555.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can computers be creative, or even disappointed?Robert J. Sternberg - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):553-554.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Individual differences, developmental changes, and social context.Dean Keith Simonton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):552-553.
  • Respecting the phenomenology of human creativity.Victor A. Shames & John F. Kihlstrom - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):551-552.
  • Representation and desire: A philosophical error with consequences for theory-of-mind research.Eric Schwitzgebel - 1999 - Philosophical Psychology 12 (2):157-180.
    This paper distinguishes two conceptions of representation at work in the philosophical literature. On the first, "contentive" conception (found, for example, in Searle and Fodor), something is a representation, roughly, if it has "propositional content". On the second, "indicative" conception (found, for example, in Dretske), representations must not only have content but also have the function of indicating something about the world. Desire is representational on the first view but not on the second. This paper argues that philosophers and psychologists (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Creativity: Metarules and emergent systems.Jonathan Rowe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):550-551.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Imagery and creativity.Klaus Rehkämper - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):550-550.
  • Creativity is in the mind of the creator.Ashwin Ram, Eric Domeshek, Linda Wills, Nancy Nersessian & Janet Kolodner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):549-549.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Computational creativity: What place for literature?Jörgen Pind - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):547-548.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The generative-rules definition of creativity.Joseph O'Rourke - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):547-547.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Imitation, Inspiration, and Creation: Cognitive Process of Creative Drawing by Copying Others' Artworks.Takeshi Okada & Kentaro Ishibashi - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (7):1804-1837.
    To investigate the cognitive processes underlying creative inspiration, we tested the extent to which viewing or copying prior examples impacted creative output in art. In Experiment 1, undergraduates made drawings under three conditions: copying an artist's drawing, then producing an original drawing; producing an original drawing without having seen another's work; and copying another artist's work, then reproducing that artist's style independently. We discovered that through copying unfamiliar abstract drawings, participants were able to produce creative drawings qualitatively different from the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • How Language Programs the Mind.Gary Lupyan & Benjamin Bergen - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (1):408-424.
    Many animals can be trained to perform novel tasks. People, too, can be trained, but sometime in early childhood people transition from being trainable to something qualitatively more powerful—being programmable. We argue that such programmability constitutes a leap in the way that organisms learn, interact, and transmit knowledge, and that what facilitates or enables this programmability is the learning and use of language. We then examine how language programs the mind and argue that it does so through the manipulation of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • How Language Programs the Mind.Gary Lupyan & Benjamin Bergen - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (2):408-424.
    Many animals can be trained to perform novel tasks. People, too, can be trained, but sometime in early childhood people transition from being trainable to something qualitatively more powerful—being programmable. We argue that such programmability constitutes a leap in the way that organisms learn, interact, and transmit knowledge, and that what facilitates or enables this programmability is the learning and use of language. We then examine how language programs the mind and argue that it does so through the manipulation of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Verbal and Figural Creativity in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development.Anat Kasirer, Esther Adi-Japha & Nira Mashal - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
  • Looking for'Constraints'in Infants'Perceptual-Cognitive Development.Julie C. Rutkowska - 1991 - Mind and Language 6 (3):215-238.
  • Conscious thought processes and creativity.Maria F. Ippolito - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):546-547.
  • The historical basis of scientific discovery.Gerd Grasshoff - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):545-546.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Creativity theory: Detail and testability.K. J. Gilhooly - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):544-545.
  • Structural Constraints on Cognitive Development: Introduction to a Special Issue of Cognitive Science.Rochel Gelman - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):3-9.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   64 citations  
  • Art for art's sake.Alan Garnham - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):543-544.
    This piece is a commentary on a precis of Maggie Boden's book "The creative mind" published in Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The birth of an idea.Liane M. Gabora - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):543-543.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Day in the Life of a Meme.Liane Gabora - 1996 - Philosophica 57 (1):53-90.
    Like the information patterns that evolve through. biological processes, mental representations or memes evolve through adaptive exploration and transformation of an information space through variation, selection, and transmission. However since memes do not contain instructions for their replication our brains do it for them, strategically, guided by a fitness landscape that reflects both internal drives and a worldview that forms through meme assimilation. This paper presents a tentative model for how an individual becomes a meme evolving agent via the emergence (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Creativity, madness, and extra strong Al.K. W. M. Fulford - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):542-543.
  • What about everyday creativity?Nick V. Flor - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):540-542.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Creative thinking presupposes the capacity for thought.James H. Fetzer - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):539-540.
  • Common Object Representations for Visual Production and Recognition.Judith E. Fan, Daniel L. K. Yamins & Nicholas B. Turk-Browne - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (8):2670-2698.
    Production and comprehension have long been viewed as inseparable components of language. The study of vision, by contrast, has centered almost exclusively on comprehension. Here we investigate drawing—the most basic form of visual production. How do we convey concepts in visual form, and how does refining this skill, in turn, affect recognition? We developed an online platform for collecting large amounts of drawing and recognition data, and applied a deep convolutional neural network model of visual cortex trained only on natural (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Common Object Representations for Visual Production and Recognition.Judith E. Fan, Daniel L. K. Yamins & Nicholas B. Turk-Browne - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (8):2670-2698.
    Production and comprehension have long been viewed as inseparable components of language. The study of vision, by contrast, has centered almost exclusively on comprehension. Here we investigate drawing—the most basic form of visual production. How do we convey concepts in visual form, and how does refining this skill, in turn, affect recognition? We developed an online platform for collecting large amounts of drawing and recognition data, and applied a deep convolutional neural network model of visual cortex trained only on natural (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Goals, analogy, and the social constraints of scientific discovery.Kevin Dunbar & Lisa M. Baker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):538-539.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Review of Mohan Matthen-Seeing, Doing, and Knowing: A Philosophical Theory of Sense Perception. [REVIEW]Dustin Stokes - 2006 - British Journal of Aesthetics 46 (3):323-325.
  • Computation: Part of the problem of creativity.Merlin Donald - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):537-538.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Creativity, combination, and cognition.Terry Dartnall - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):537-537.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Drawings of Representational Images by Upper Paleolithic Humans and their Absence in Neanderthals Reflect Historical Differences in Hunting Wary Game.Richard G. Coss - 2017 - Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture 1 (2):15-38.
    One characteristic of the transition from the Middle Paleolithic to the Upper Paleolithic in Europe was the emergence of representational charcoal drawings and engravings by Aurignacian and Gravettian artists. European Neanderthals never engaged in representational drawing during the Middle and Early Upper Paleolithic, a property that might reflect less developed visuomotor coordination. This article postulates a causal relationship between an evolved ability of anatomically modern humans to throw spears accurately while hunting and their ability to draw representational images from working (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cognizer's innards: A psychological and philosophical perspective on the development of thought.Andy Clark & Annette Karmiloff-Smith - 1993 - Mind and Language 8 (4):487-519.
  • On doing the impossible.Robert L. Campbell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):535-537.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Analogy programs and creativity.Bruce D. Burns - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):535-535.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What is the difference between real creativity and mere novelty?Alan Bundy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):533-534.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Lady Lovelace had it right: Computers originate nothing.Selmer Bringsjord - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):532-533.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Précis of The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms.Margaret A. Boden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):519-531.
    What is creativity? One new idea may be creative, whereas another is merely new: What's the difference? And how is creativity possible? These questions about human creativity can be answered, at least in outline, using computational concepts. There are two broad types of creativity, improbabilist and impossibilist. Improbabilist creativity involves novel combinations of familiar ideas. A deeper type involves METCS: the mapping, exploration, and transformation of conceptual spaces. It is impossibilist, in that ideas may be generated which – with respect (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations