Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Ethologists do not study human evolution.S. L. Washburn - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):49-49.
  • Pattern proliferation in teleological behaviorism.Bruce N. Waller - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):145-146.
  • Not “pain and behavior” but pain in behavior.Patrick D. Wall - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):73-73.
  • How general is a general theory of reinforcement?Stephen F. Walker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):154-155.
  • A brief history of connectionism and its psychological implications.S. F. Walker - 1990 - AI and Society 4 (1):17-38.
    Critics of the computational connectionism of the last decade suggest that it shares undesirable features with earlier empiricist or associationist approaches, and with behaviourist theories of learning. To assess the accuracy of this charge the works of earlier writers are examined for the presence of such features, and brief accounts of those found are given for Herbert Spencer, William James and the learning theorists Thorndike, Pavlov and Hull. The idea that cognition depends on associative connections among large networks of neurons (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Some logical fallacies in the classical ethological point of view.Douglas Wahlsten - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):48-49.
  • Presupposing Weber's Law: Theory without independent confirmation is circular.Mark Wagner - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):312-313.
  • Behavioral and neuro-endocrine influences in homeostasis.J. D. Vincent - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):122-122.
  • Contextual determinants of pain reactions.Charles J. Vierck & Brian Y. Cooper - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):314-315.
  • On putting the cart before the horse: Taking perception seriously in unified theories of cognition.Kim J. Vicente & Alex Kirlik - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):461-462.
  • A cognitive process shell.Steven A. Vere - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):460-461.
  • Reinforcement or maximization?William Vaughan - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):405-405.
  • Lack of “fixed set-points” in fluid homeostasis does not argue for learned satiety factors in drinking.Dennis A. VanderWeele - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):121-122.
  • Ethology versus sociobiology: competitive displays.Pierre L. van den Berghe - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):46-48.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does conflict help or hurt cognitive control? Initial evidence for an inverted U-shape relationship between perceived task difficulty and conflict adaptation.Henk van Steenbergen, Guido P. H. Band & Bernhard Hommel - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  • Bi-stability Of Emotions And Motivations: An evolutionary consequence of the open-ended capacity for learning.P. P. van der Molen - 1984 - Acta Biotheoretica 33 (4):227-251.
    Species, endowed with an open-ended capacity for learning, which is one of the highest evolutionary achievements,will profit most from this ability, if they are urged one way or other to invest any surplus of energy in expanding and refining their behavioural repertoire and in adapting it to prevailing circumstances, while incurring as little risk and stress as possible.It is therefore argued that an open-ended capacity for learning is maximally adding to survival if paired to two distinct tendencies: a tendency to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On models and mechanisms.William R. Uttal - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):459-460.
  • Internal representations and indeterminacy: A skeptical view.William R. Uttal - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):392-393.
  • Progress and degeneration in the 'IQ debate' (II).Peter Urbach - 1974 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 25 (3):235-259.
  • A differentiated view of Weber's Law.Christopher W. Tyler - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):311-312.
  • The reign of pain fails mainly in the brain.Dennis C. Turk & Peter Salovey - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):72-73.
  • Functions and effects of Pavlovian stimuli.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):394-398.
  • Relations among components and processes of memory.Endel Tulving - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):257.
  • Précis of Elements of episodic memory.Endel Tulving - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):223.
  • Looking for nodes and edges.Arnold Trehub - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):650-651.
  • Unified theories and theories that mimic each other's predictions.James T. Townsend - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):458-459.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Stimulus correlations in complex operant settings.François Tonneau - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):393-394.
  • Further choices for molar theory.François Tonneau - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):145-145.
    The target article extends molar behaviorism in two positive ways: beyond average aggregates and beyond restricted laws of Although a molar framework based on purely overt events shows promise for advancing behavior theory, Rachlin's specific form of teleological behaviorism is in need of clarification.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Well-fed organisms still need feedback.Michael Tomasello & Catherine E. Snow - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):475.
  • Sensory Analysis: The question of balance.David L. Tomko - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):311-311.
  • Contingency: Effects of symmetry of choice responses.Arthur Tomie - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):476.
  • The human being as a bumbling optimalist: A psychologist's viewpoint.Masanao Toda - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):235-235.
  • The psychobiology and biocybernetics of thirst; Invaluable data and concepts for future theory and model construction.F. M. Toates - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):125-136.
  • Models, yes; homunculus, no.Frederick M. Toates - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):650.
  • Feedforward and feedbackward.Frederick Toates - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):474.
  • Talking with Alex.William Timberlake - 2003 - Semiotica 2003 (146):439-471.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Feedforward and feedback processes in learning: The importance of appetitive structure.William Timberlake - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):472.
  • Bliss points and utility functions.William Timberlake - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):404-405.
  • Animal-centered models of reinforcement.William Timberlake - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):153-154.
  • Just how does ecphory work?Guy Tiberghien - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):255.
  • The law of effect: Contingency or contiguity.David R. Thomas - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):470.
  • The law of obligation is insufficient.Claudia R. Thompson - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):471.
  • Observing and information: Bad news is better than no news – but spare us the details.Roger K. R. Thompson & Stephen Wilcox - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):717.
  • Are some mental states public events?Nicholas S. Thompson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):662-663.
  • Are radical and cognitive behaviorism incompatible?Roger K. Thomas - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):650.
  • The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching.Esther Thelen, Gregor Schöner, Christian Scheier & Linda B. Smith - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1):1-34.
    The overall goal of this target article is to demonstrate a mechanism for an embodied cognition. The particular vehicle is a much-studied, but still widely debated phenomenon seen in 7–12 month-old-infants. In Piaget's classic “A-not-B error,” infants who have successfully uncovered a toy at location “A” continue to reach to that location even after they watch the toy hidden in a nearby location “B.” Here, we question the traditional explanations of the error as an indicator of infants' concepts of objects (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   122 citations  
  • Maximization and self-control.Richard H. Thaler - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):403-404.
  • “Mental way stations” in contemporary theories of animal learning.William S. Terry - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):649.
  • Implications of recent research in conditioning for the conditioning model of neurosis.William S. Terry - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):183-184.
  • Animal versus human minds.H. S. Terrace - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):391-392.