36 found
Order:
Disambiguations
Mark S. Seidenberg [35]Mark Seidenberg [1]
  1.  28
    A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming.Mark S. Seidenberg & James L. McClelland - 1989 - Psychological Review 96 (4):523-568.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   415 citations  
  2.  19
    Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains.David C. Plaut, James L. McClelland, Mark S. Seidenberg & Karalyn Patterson - 1996 - Psychological Review 103 (1):56-115.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   191 citations  
  3.  30
    The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution.Maryellen C. MacDonald, Neal J. Pearlmutter & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1994 - Psychological Review 101 (4):676-703.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   185 citations  
  4.  27
    Signing behavior in apes: A critical review.Mark S. Seidenberg & Laura A. Petitto - 1979 - Cognition 7 (2):177-215.
  5.  87
    Letting structure emerge: connectionist and dynamical systems approaches to cognition.James L. McClelland, Matthew M. Botvinick, David C. Noelle, David C. Plaut, Timothy T. Rogers, Mark S. Seidenberg & Linda B. Smith - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (8):348-356.
  6.  27
    Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models.Michael W. Harm & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1999 - Psychological Review 106 (3):491-528.
  7.  31
    Computing the Meanings of Words in Reading: Cooperative Division of Labor Between Visual and Phonological Processes.Michael W. Harm & Mark S. Seidenberg - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (3):662-720.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   67 citations  
  8.  65
    On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning.Ken McRae, Virginia R. de Sa & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1997 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 126 (2):99-130.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   65 citations  
  9.  31
    Distributional structure in language: Contributions to noun–verb difficulty differences in infant word recognition.Jon A. Willits, Mark S. Seidenberg & Jenny R. Saffran - 2014 - Cognition 132 (3):429-436.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  10.  25
    The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1985 - Cognition 19 (1):1-30.
  11.  19
    Constraining models of word recognition.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1985 - Cognition 20 (2):169-190.
  12.  20
    Steps toward an ethological science.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):377-377.
  13.  35
    A Probabilistic Constraints Approach to Language Acquisition and Processing.Mark S. Seidenberg & Maryellen C. MacDonald - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):569-588.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  14.  34
    On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia.Franklin R. Manis, Mark S. Seidenberg, Lisa M. Doi, Catherine McBride-Chang & Alan Petersen - 1996 - Cognition 58 (2):157-195.
  15.  66
    Specific language impairment: a deficit in grammar or processing?Marc F. Joanisse & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1998 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 (7):240-247.
  16.  53
    Quasiregularity and Its Discontents: The Legacy of the Past Tense Debate.Mark S. Seidenberg & David C. Plaut - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1190-1228.
    Rumelhart and McClelland's chapter about learning the past tense created a degree of controversy extraordinary even in the adversarial culture of modern science. It also stimulated a vast amount of research that advanced the understanding of the past tense, inflectional morphology in English and other languages, the nature of linguistic representations, relations between language and other phenomena such as reading and object recognition, the properties of artificial neural networks, and other topics. We examine the impact of the Rumelhart and McClelland (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  17.  53
    Rules Versus Statistics: Insights From a Highly Inflected Language.Jelena Mirković, Mark S. Seidenberg & Marc F. Joanisse - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (4):638-681.
    Inflectional morphology has been taken as a paradigmatic example of rule-governed grammatical knowledge (Pinker, 1999). The plausibility of this claim may be related to the fact that it is mainly based on studies of English, which has a very simple inflectional system. We examined the representation of inflectional morphology in Serbian, which encodes number, gender, and case for nouns. Linguists standardly characterize this system as a complex set of rules, with disagreements about their exact form. We present analyses of a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  18.  12
    More words but still no lexicon: Reply to Besner et al. (1990).Mark S. Seidenberg & James L. McClelland - 1990 - Psychological Review 97 (3):447-452.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  19.  25
    Efforts to Encourage Multidisciplinarity in the Cognitive Science Society.James G. Greeno, William J. Clancey, Clayton Lewis, Mark Seidenberg, Sharon Derry, Morton Ann Gernsbacher, Patrick Langley, Michael Shafto, Dedre Gentner, Alan Lesgold & Colleen M. Seifert - 1998 - Cognitive Science 22 (1):131-132.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  20. Consistency effects in the generation of past tense morphology.Mark S. Seidenberg & Maggie Bruck - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (6):522-522.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  21.  12
    Language and connectionism: the developing interface.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1994 - Cognition 50 (1-3):385-401.
  22.  7
    Preface.Morten H. Christiansen, Nick Chater & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):415-415.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  23.  46
    Writing systems: Not optimal, but good enough.Mark S. Seidenberg & Ram Frost - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (5):305.
    Languages and writing systems result from satisfying multiple constraints related to learning, comprehension, production, and their biological bases. Orthographies are not optimal because these constraints often conflict, with further deviations due to accidents of history and geography. Things tend to even out because writing systems and the languages they represent exhibit systematic trade-offs between orthographic depth and morphological complexity.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  24.  43
    Impact of dialect use on a basic component of learning to read.Megan C. Brown, Daragh E. Sibley, Julie A. Washington, Timothy T. Rogers, Jan R. Edwards, Maryellen C. MacDonald & Mark S. Seidenberg - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  24
    Computational bases of two types of developmental dyslexia.Michael W. Harm & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1996 - In Garrison W. Cottrell (ed.), Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 18--364.
  26. Using connectionist networks to examine the role of prior constraints in human learning.Michael Harm, Lori Altmann & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1994 - In Ashwin Ram & Kurt Eiselt (eds.), Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum. pp. 392--396.
  27. Acquisition and representation of grammatical categories: Grammatical gender in a connectionist network.Jelena Mirkovic, Mark S. Seidenberg & Maryellen C. MacDonald - 2008 - In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 1954--1959.
  28.  23
    Distinguishing literal from metaphorical applications of Bayesian approaches.Timothy T. Rogers & Mark S. Seidenberg - 2011 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (4):211-212.
    We distinguish between literal and metaphorical applications of Bayesian models. When intended literally, an isomorphism exists between the elements of representation assumed by the rational analysis and the mechanism that implements the computation. Thus, observation of the implementation can externally validate assumptions underlying the rational analysis. In other applications, no such isomorphism exists, so it is not clear how the assumptions that allow a Bayesian model to fit data can be independently validated.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29. Books etcetera-talking nets: An oral history of neural networks.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1999 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3 (3):120-121.
  30.  11
    Box 1. Main types of morphological structure.Mark S. Seidenberg & Laura M. Gonnerman - 2000 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (9):353-361.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31. Connectionist models of reading.Mark S. Seidenberg - 2009 - In Gareth Gaskell (ed.), Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Oxford University Press.
  32.  27
    Explanatory adequacy and models of word recognition.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):724-726.
  33. Evidence from great apes concerning the biological bases of language.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1986 - In William Demopoulos (ed.), Language Learning and Concept Acquisition. Ablex.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  34.  21
    Lexicon as module.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):31-32.
  35.  25
    Writing systems: Not optimal, but good enough – Erratum.Mark S. Seidenberg - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (6):467-467.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  22
    Error, error everywhere: A look at megastudies of word reading.Daragh E. Sibley, Christopher T. Kello & Mark S. Seidenberg - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. pp. 1036--1041.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark