Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. An ecological approach toward a unified theory of learning.William R. Charlesworth - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):142-143.
  • On the careful use of ecological models.Thomas Caraco - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):680-681.
  • On goals, perceptions, and self-control.Charles S. Carver - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):681-682.
  • Is the distinction between Type I and Type II behaviors related to the effects of septal lesions?Neil R. Carlson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):479-479.
  • Fitness, currencies, and models.Thomas Caraco - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):133-133.
  • Variations of reflex parameters and their implications for the control of movements.Charles Capaday & Richard B. Stein - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):600-602.
  • Folk psychology redux.Linnda R. Caporael - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):302-303.
  • Levels of organization, selection, and information storage in biological and social evaluation.Donald T. Campbell - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):236-237.
  • How is a toad not like a bug?Jeffrey M. Camhi - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):371-372.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ambivalently held group-optimizing predispositions.Donald T. Campbell & John B. Gatewood - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):614-614.
  • State, function, and optimization.William A. Calder - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):131-133.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Invertebrate central pattern generators: modeling and complexity.Ronald L. Calabrese - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):542-543.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can the decomposition of attention clarify some clinical issues?Enoch Callaway - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):477-479.
  • Lire la communication-monde au XXIe siècle.Bertrand Cabedoche - 2022 - University of Ottawa Press.
    "Consacrée dans des intitulés de cours, mobilisée dans les nomenclatures des organismes internationaux, prometteuse de débroussaillages très vites décevants ou de synthèses faussement structurantes, l'appellation objectivante communication internationale ne présente aucune valeur scientifique, sinon en tant qu'objet de recherche. Pour autant, la référence produit des effets de sens, qu'il est urgent de mettre en perspective, tant elle prête le jeu à des constructions discursives à géométrie variable, en fonction des intérêts croisés et souvent masqués d'acteurs de plus en plus nombreux (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The mobility gradient: Useful, general, falsifiable?John A. Byers - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):270-271.
  • Behavioral problems related to the interpretation of brain rhythms.György Buzsáki, Robert L. Isaacson & John H. Hannigan - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):477-477.
  • Toward an empirical foundation for evolutionary psychology.David M. Buss - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):301-302.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Looking inside monkey minds: Milestone or millstone.Gordon M. Burghardt - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):150-151.
  • Group selection and the group mind in science.Gordon M. Burghardt - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):613-613.
  • Ethology and operant psychology.Gordon M. Burghardt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):683-684.
  • Ethology and the development of sex and gender identity in non-human primates.Frances D. Burton - 1977 - Acta Biotheoretica 26 (1):1-18.
    The current view that behaviour which is manifest in non-human primates forms a baseline for human behaviours is examined with special reference to the development of gender determination. A review of 21 non-human primate societies suggests that the behaviour of the sexes relates to assumption and occupation of societal roles defined by the local group. The significance of these findings for the human condition is discussed.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Developmental creationism.Gordon M. Burghardt - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):632-632.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology: In defense of open range; don't fence me in.Theodore H. Bullock - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):383.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Adaptation and self-organization in primate societies.Bernard Thierry - 1997 - Diogenes 45 (180):39-71.
    The primary method by which science endeavors to order the world is the analytic approach, consistent with Cartesian principles of dividing the problem in as many sections as required for an optimal solution, and progressing from the simplest to the most complex reasoning. When the interactions among the various elements of the system being studied are minimal, such a procedure indeed makes it possible to formulate laws that describe chains of causality. However, when the variables are interdependent and linked by (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Neuroethology: An overnarrow definition can become a source of dogmatism.Ulrich Bässler - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):382.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cost–benefit models and the evolution of behavior.Jerram L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):682-682.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • After the sensory analysers: Problems with concepts and terminology.D. M. Broom - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):370-371.
  • The consequences of group selection in a domain without genetic input: Culture.C. Loring Brace - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):611-612.
  • Metaphors and mechanisms in vehicle-based selection theory.Michael Bradie - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):612-612.
  • Dividing up aggression and considerations in studying the physiological substrates of these phenomena.Paul F. Brain - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):216-216.
  • A theory in need of defense.Marc N. Branch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):678-679.
  • The motivation and/or reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Robert C. Bolles - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):677-678.
  • On a clear day you can see behavior.Robert C. Bolles - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):619-620.
  • New elements of a theory of mind in wild chimpanzees.Christophe Boesch - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):149-150.
  • Snake oil and the modeling process in neurobiology.Gene D. Block - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):541-542.
  • Neurobehavioral systems for attack and defense.Robert J. Blanchard & D. Caroline Blanchard - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):215-216.
  • A new experimental analysis of behavior – one for all behavior.D. Caroline Blanchard, Robert J. Blanchard & Kevin J. Flannelly - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):681-682.
  • How adaptive behavior is produced: a perceptual-motivational alternative to response reinforcements.Dalbir Bindra - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (1):41-52.
  • “Other minds than ours”: a controversial discussion on the limits and possibilities of comparative psychology in the light of C. Lloyd Morgan’s work.Martin Böhnert & Christopher Hilbert - 2018 - History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 40 (3):44.
    C. Lloyd Morgan is mostly known for Morgan’s canon, still a popular and frequently quoted principle in comparative psychology and ethology. There has been a fair amount of debate on the canon’s interpretation, function, and value regarding the research on animal minds, usually referring to it as an isolated principle. In this paper we rather shed light on Morgan’s overall scientific program and his vision for comparative psychology. We argue that within his program Morgan identified crucial conceptual, ontological, and methodical (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • In search of the theoretical basis of motor control.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):626-638.
  • Cerebellar contributions to response selection.Gary G. Berntson - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):214-215.
  • Avoidance theory: Solutions or more problems?Philip J. Bersh - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):676-677.
  • Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):585-599.
  • Niche Construction and Cognitive Evolution.Benjamin Kerr - 2007 - Biological Theory 2 (3):250-262.
    Despite the fact that animal behavior involves a particularly powerful form of niche construction, few researchers have considered how the environmental impact of behavior may feed back to influence the evolution of the cognitive underpinnings of behavior. I explore a model that explicitly incorporates niche construction while tracking cognitive evolution. Agents and their stimuli are modeled as coevolving populations. The agents are born with “weights” attached to behaviors in a repertoire. Further, these agents are able to change these weights based (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Is a behaviorist's approach sufficient for understanding the brain?Thomas L. Bennett - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):476-477.
  • Social cognition: Exchanging and sharing information on the run. [REVIEW]Marc Bekoff - 1999 - Erkenntnis 51 (1):617-632.
    In this essay I consider various aspects of the rapidly growing field of cognitive ethology, concentrating mainly on evolutionary and comparative discussion of the notion of intentionality. I am not concerned with consciousness, per se, for a concentration on consciousness deflects attention from other, and in many cases more interesting, problems in the study of animal cognition. I consider how, when, where, and (attempt to discuss) why individuals from different taxa exchange social information concerning their beliefs, desires, and goals. My (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Description and explanation: A plea for plurality.Marc Bekoff - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):269-270.
  • Birdsong and the “problem” of nature and nurture: Endless chirping about inadequate evidence or merely singing the blues about inevitable biases in, and limitations of, human inference?Marc Bekoff - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):631-631.
  • The environment modulates the mobility gradient, temporally if not sequentially.Charles H. M. Beck - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):268-269.
  • The proximate/ultimate distinction in the multiple careers of Ernst Mayr.John Beatty - 1994 - Biology and Philosophy 9 (3):333-356.
    Ernst Mayr''s distinction between ultimate and proximate causes is justly considered a major contribution to philosophy of biology. But how did Mayr come to this philosophical distinction, and what role did it play in his earlier scientific work? I address these issues by dividing Mayr''s work into three careers or phases: 1) Mayr the naturalist/researcher, 2) Mayr the representative of and spokesman for evolutionary biology and systematics, and more recently 3) Mayr the historian and philosopher of biology. If we want (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations