Results for ' stimulus differences'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  29
    Multidimensional stimulus differences and accuracy of discrimination.Charles W. Eriksen & Harold W. Hake - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 50 (3):153.
  2.  15
    Stimulus differences in discrimination learning.R. A. Champion & C. D. Standish - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (2):78.
  3.  31
    Studies in decision: I. Decision-time, relative frequency of judgment and subjective confidence as related to physical stimulus difference.L. Festinger - 1943 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 32 (4):291.
  4.  23
    Relation between disjunctive reaction time and stimulus difference.Dwight W. Curtis, Manley A. Paulos & Stanley J. Rule - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 99 (2):167.
  5.  16
    Primary stimulus generalization in discrimination learning as a function of number of trials and incidental cue differences.Leopold O. Walder - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (2):178.
  6.  11
    Individual differences in interference from stimulus similarity.Willard N. Runquist & David Blackmore - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 87 (1):141.
  7.  19
    Sex differences in adaptation of the GSR under repeated applications of a visual stimulus.H. D. Kimmel & E. Kimmel - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (5):536.
  8.  19
    Strain differences in simple operant barpress acquisition to an auditory stimulus by rats.Gordon M. Harrington - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 13 (3):163-164.
  9.  16
    Primary stimulus generalization under different percentages of reinforcement in eyelid conditioning.William E. Vandament & Louis Price - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (2):162.
  10.  23
    Stimulus generalization following different methods of training.Daniel B. Reinhold & Charles C. Perkins Jr - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (6):423.
  11.  10
    Stimulus-Specific Individual Differences in Holistic Perception of Mooney Faces.Teresa Canas-Bajo & David Whitney - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  92
    Comparing different stimulus configurations for population receptive field mapping in human fMRI.Ivan Alvarez, Benjamin de Haas, Chris A. Clark, Geraint Rees & D. Samuel Schwarzkopf - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  13.  23
    Stimulus selection and meaningfulness at different stages of paired-associate learning.Franklin M. Berry, Charles E. Joubert & Alfred A. Baumeister - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 95 (1):189.
  14. Differences in the symbolic distance effect due to comparison task and stimulus surface-structure.Pc Amrhein & J. Theios - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (6):519-519.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  9
    Sharing Different Reference Frames: How Stimulus Setup and Task Setup Shape Egocentric and Allocentric Simon Effects.Pamela Baess, Tom Weber & Christina Bermeitinger - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  13
    Effect of different stimulus frequencies on discrimination learning with probabilistic reinforcement.Juliet Popper Shaffer - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (3):265.
  17.  22
    Effect of delayed conditioned stimulus termination on extinction of an avoidance response following different termination conditions during acquisition.Allen C. Israel, Vernon T. Devine, Margaret A. O'Dea & Mark E. Hamdi - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (2):360.
  18.  31
    Audio-visual onset differences are used to determine syllable identity for ambiguous audio-visual stimulus pairs.Sanne ten Oever, Alexander Sack, Katherine L. Wheat, Nina Bien & Nienke van Atteveldt - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  19.  48
    Stimulus-category competition, inhibition, and affective devaluation: a novel account of the uncanny valley.Anne E. Ferrey, Tyler J. Burleigh & Mark J. Fenske - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:92507.
    Stimuli that resemble humans, but are not perfectly human-like, are disliked compared to distinctly human and nonhuman stimuli. Accounts of this “Uncanny Valley” effect often focus on how changes in human resemblance can evoke different emotional responses. We present an alternate account based on the novel hypothesis that the Uncanny Valley is not directly related to ‘human-likeness’ per se, but instead reflects a more general form of stimulus devaluation that occurs when inhibition is triggered to resolve conflict between competing (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  20.  25
    From Stimulus to Science.W. V. Quine - 1995 - Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
    W. V. Quine is one of the most eminent philosophers alive today. Now in his mid-eighties he has produced a sharp, sprightly book that encapsulates the whole of his philosophical enterprise, including his thinking on all the key components of his epistemological stance--especially the value of logic and mathematics. New readers of Quine may have to go slowly, fathoming for themselves the richness that past readers already know lies between these elegant lines. For the faithful there is much to ponder. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   64 citations  
  21.  16
    Effects of practice with controlled stimulus pairs on same-different judgments.John D. Williams - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 96 (1):73.
  22.  19
    The Effect of Stressor Level Grading on the Stimulus Seeking Behavior of Rats Differing in Emotional Reactivity1.Jan Matysiak & Dominika Farley - 2008 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 39 (2):98-103.
    The Effect of Stressor Level Grading on the Stimulus Seeking Behavior of Rats Differing in Emotional Reactivity1 A natural disaster — such as a flood — is a sequence of events: swollen water level leading to the flooding of homesteads — primary stressor and later environmental consequences — secondary stressor syndrome. In order to be valid, an experimental model must ensure similarity of the stress-evoked behavioral symptoms. The most frequently administered behavioral tests measure exploratory behavior in the broad sense. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  26
    Pain measurement by the radiant heat method: individual differences in pain sensitivity, the effects of skin temperature, and stimulus duration.James E. Birren, Roland C. Casperson & Jack Botwinick - 1951 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 41 (6):419.
  24.  4
    Do gaze and non-gaze stimuli trigger different spatial interference effects? It depends on stimulus perceivability.Zhe Chen, Rebecca H. Thomas & Makayla S. Chen - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Among the studies on the perception of gaze vs. non-gaze stimuli, some have shown that the two types of stimuli trigger different patterns of attentional effects, while others have reported no such differences. In three experiments, we investigated the role of stimulus perceivability in spatial interference effects when the targets were gaze vs. non-gaze stimuli. We used a spatial Stroop task that required participants to make a speeded response to the direction indicated by the targets located on the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  16
    Temporal dynamics in attention bias: effects of sex differences, task timing parameters, and stimulus valence.Joshua M. Carlson, Jacob S. Aday & Denis Rubin - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (6):1271-1276.
    ABSTRACTNew methods of calculating indices from the dot-probe task measure temporal dynamics in attention bias or fluctuations in attention bias towards and away from emotional stimuli over time. H...
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  26.  23
    Learning of interval concepts: I. Effects of differences in stimulus weights.Charles N. Uhl - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (3):264.
  27.  42
    Gibson' S ecological optics: Consequences of a different stimulus description.Ulric Neisser - 1977 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 7 (1):17–28.
  28. Perception needs modular stimulus-control.Anders Nes - 2023 - Synthese 201 (6):1-30.
    Perceptual processes differ from cognitive, this paper argues, in functioning to be causally controlled by proximal stimuli, and being modular, at least in a modest sense that excludes their being isotropic in Jerry Fodor's sense. This claim agrees with such theorists as Jacob Beck and Ben Phillips that a function of stimulus-control is needed for perceptual status. In support of this necessity claim, I argue, inter alia, that E.J. Green's recent architectural account misclassifies processes deploying knowledge of grammar as (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29.  9
    Stimulus intensity, site of stimulation, and individual reactivity as determinants of the energy threshold for pricking pain.Donald J. Dillon - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (4):559.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  10
    Conflict-Elicited Negative Evaluations of Neutral Stimuli: Testing Overt Responses and Stimulus-Frequency Differences as Critical Side Conditions.Florian Goller, Alexandra Kroiss & Ulrich Ansorge - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31.  16
    Biologically primed acquisition of aversions and association of expected stimulus pairs: Two different forms of learning.Alfons Hamm - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):301-302.
    The present commentary emphasizes that the acquisition of fear always involves complex changes in several quasi-independent response systems. Stimulus-specific electrodermal response differentiation as well as the bias to overestimate the belongingness of certain stimulus pairs mainly indicates cognitive processes of selective orienting and attention. Emotion, however, also involves the activation of subcortical motivational circuits. Why certain stimuli acquire rapid access to these basic motivational systems is not explained by the expectancy bias model.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  22
    Speed and Lateral Inhibition of Stimulus Processing Contribute to Individual Differences in Stroop-Task Performance.Marnix Naber, Anneke Vedder, Stephen B. R. E. Brown & Sander Nieuwenhuis - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  33.  6
    Stimulus valence moderates self-learning.Parnian Jalalian, Saga Svensson, Marius Golubickis, Yadvi Sharma & C. Neil Macrae - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Self-relevance has been demonstrated to impair instrumental learning. Compared to unfamiliar symbols associated with a friend, analogous stimuli linked with the self are learned more slowly. What is not yet understood, however, is whether this effect extends beyond arbitrary stimuli to material with intrinsically meaningful properties. Take, for example, stimulus valence an established moderator of self-bias. Does the desirability of to-be-learned material influence self-learning? Here, in conjunction with computational modelling (i.e. Reinforcement Learning Drift Diffusion Model analysis), a probabilistic selection (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  26
    Dissociation of hemifield reaction time differences from verbal stimulus directionality.Ami Isseroff, Amiram Carmon & Israel Nachshon - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (1):145.
  35.  26
    Thematic and taxonomic priming effects at different length stimulus onset asynchronies.Filippova Daria & Winskel Heather - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  14
    Response suppression during stimuli temporally different from the prefood stimulus.Anne S. Bice & John de Lorge - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11 (6):377-379.
  37.  12
    The effect of uncertainty in stimulus perception on same-different judgments.David Navon - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12 (6):411-414.
  38.  26
    Only stimulus energy affects the detectability of visual forms and objects.Muriel Boucart & Claude Bonnet - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (5):415-417.
    A detection task was performed using different pictographic representations of objects in order to test the hypothesis that high-level information (familiarity) may influence detection thresholds. The stimuli were five versions of forms: outline drawings of objects, silhouettes, and three fragmented versions of forms derived from the outlines. The stimuli varied on two parameters: their nameability (easily nameable, hardly nameable, and not nameable) as assessed by a naming task, and their energy content as assessed by a two-dimensional fast-Fourier transform. The greatest (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  11
    Conceptual recombination and stimulus-independence in non-human animals.Laura Danón - 2022 - Theoria: Revista de Teoría, Historia y Fundamentos de la Ciencia 37 (3):309-330.
    Camp (2009) distinguishes two varieties of conceptual recombination. One of them is full-blown or (as I prefer to call it) spontaneous recombination. The other is causal-counterfactual recombination. She suggests that while human animals recombine their concepts in a full-blown way, many non-human animals are capable of conceptual recombinability but only of the causal-counterfactual kind. In this paper, I argue that there is conceptual space to draw further sub-distinctions on how different animals may recombine their concepts. More specifically, I propose to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  40.  17
    Audio-Visual Causality and Stimulus Reliability Affect Audio-Visual Synchrony Perception.Shao Li, Qi Ding, Yichen Yuan & Zhenzhu Yue - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12:629996.
    People can discriminate the synchrony between audio-visual scenes. However, the sensitivity of audio-visual synchrony perception can be affected by many factors. Using a simultaneity judgment task, the present study investigated whether the synchrony perception of complex audio-visual stimuli was affected by audio-visual causality and stimulus reliability. In Experiment 1, the results showed that audio-visual causality could increase one's sensitivity to audio-visual onset asynchrony (AVOA) of both action stimuli and speech stimuli. Moreover, participants were more tolerant of AVOA of speech (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  28
    EEG Differentiation Analysis and Stimulus Set Meaningfulness.Armand Mensen, William Marshall & Giulio Tononi - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    A set of images can be considered as meaningfully different for an observer if they can be distinguished phenomenally from one another. Each phenomenal difference must be supported by some neurophysiological differences. Differentiation analysis aims to quantify neurophysiological differentiation evoked by a given set of stimuli to assess its meaningfulness to the individual observer. As a proof of concept using high-density EEG, we show increased neurophysiological differentiation for a set of natural, meaningfully different images in contrast to another set (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42.  9
    Conceptual recombination and stimulus-independence in non-human animals.Laura Danón - 2022 - Theoria: Revista de Teoría, Historia y Fundamentos de la Ciencia 37 (3):309-330.
    Camp (2009) distinguishes two varieties of conceptual recombination. One of them is full-blown or (as I prefer to call it) spontaneous recombination. The other is causal-counterfactual recombination. She suggests that while human animals recombine their concepts in a full-blown way, many non-human animals are capable of conceptual recombinability but only of the causal-counterfactual kind. In this paper, I argue that there is conceptual space to draw further sub-distinctions on how different animals may recombine their concepts. More specifically, I propose to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  43.  28
    Analysis of stimulus generalization with a psychophysical method.Eric G. Heinemann, Edward Avin, Mary A. Sullivan & Sheila Chase - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (2p1):215.
  44.  10
    The Role of Stimulus‐Specific Perceptual Fluency in Statistical Learning.Andrew Perfors & Evan Kidd - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13100.
    Humans have the ability to learn surprisingly complicated statistical information in a variety of modalities and situations, often based on relatively little input. These statistical learning (SL) skills appear to underlie many kinds of learning, but despite their ubiquity, we still do not fully understand precisely what SL is and what individual differences on SL tasks reflect. Here, we present experimental work suggesting that at least some individual differences arise from stimulus-specific variation in perceptual fluency: the ability (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  6
    The Neural Representation of a Repeated Standard Stimulus in Dyslexia.Sara D. Beach, Ola Ozernov-Palchik, Sidney C. May, Tracy M. Centanni, Tyler K. Perrachione, Dimitrios Pantazis & John D. E. Gabrieli - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    The neural representation of a repeated stimulus is the standard against which a deviant stimulus is measured in the brain, giving rise to the well-known mismatch response. It has been suggested that individuals with dyslexia have poor implicit memory for recently repeated stimuli, such as the train of standards in an oddball paradigm. Here, we examined how the neural representation of a standard emerges over repetitions, asking whether there is less sensitivity to repetition and/or less accrual of “standardness” (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  46.  24
    Symbolic Processes and Stimulus Equivalence.Ullin T. Place - 1995 - Behavior and Philosophy 23 (3-1):13 - 30.
    A symbol is defined as a species of sign. The concept of a sign coincides with Skinner's (1938) concept of a discriminative stimulus. Symbols differ from other signs in five respects: (1) They are stimuli which the organism can both respond to and produce, either as a self-directed stimulus (as in thinking) or as a stimulus for another individual with a predictably similar response from the recipient in each case. (2) they act as discriminative stimuli for the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47.  55
    Measuring consciousness: Task accuracy and awareness as sigmoid functions of stimulus duration.Kristian Sandberg, Bo Martin Bibby, Bert Timmermans, Axel Cleeremans & Morten Overgaard - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1659-1675.
    When consciousness is examined using subjective ratings, the extent to which processing is conscious or unconscious is often estimated by calculating task performance at the subjective threshold or by calculating the correlation between accuracy and awareness. However, both these methods have certain limitations. In the present article, we propose describing task accuracy and awareness as functions of stimulus intensity as suggested by Koch and Preuschoff . The estimated lag between the curves describes how much stimulus intensity must increase (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  48.  11
    Mechanisms of False Alarm in Response to Fear Stimulus: An Event-Related Potential Study.Xiai Wang, Jicheng Sun, Jinghua Yang, Shan Cheng, Cui Liu, Wendong Hu & Jin Ma - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Background and ObjectiveThere is a paucity of research that has explored “False Alarm” mechanisms. In order to remedy this deficiency in knowledge, the present study used event-related potential technology to reveal the mechanisms underlying False Alarm in response to fear stimuli.MethodsThis study selected snakes as experimental materials and the “oddball paradigm” was used to simulate the conditions of False Alarm. The mechanism underlying False Alarm was revealed by comparing cognitive processing similarities and differences between real snakes and toy snakes.ResultsEvent-related (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  4
    Influence of Stimulus Size on Simultaneous Chromatic Induction.Tama Kanematsu & Kowa Koida - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Chromatic induction is a major contextual effect of color appearance. Patterned backgrounds are known to induce strong chromatic induction effects. However, it has not been clarified whether the spatial extent of the chromatic surrounding induces a chromatic contrast or assimilation effects. In this study, we examined the influence of the width of a center line and its flanking white contour on the color appearance when the line was surrounded by chromatic backgrounds. A strong color shift was observed when the center (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  64
    On subjective back-referral and how long it takes to become conscious of a stimulus: A reinterpretation of Libet's data.Susan Pockett - 2002 - Consciousness and Cognition 11 (2):141-61.
    The original data reported by Benjamin Libet and colleagues are reinterpreted, taking into account the facilitation which is experimentally demonstrated in the first of their series of articles. It is shown that the original data equally well or better support a quite different set of conclusions from those drawn by Libet. The new conclusions are that it takes only 80 ms for stimuli to come to consciousness and that “subjective back-referral of sensations in time” to the time of the (...) does not occur. (shrink)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000