Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Bootstrapping the Mind: Analogical Processes and Symbol Systems.Dedre Gentner - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (5):752-775.
    Human cognition is striking in its brilliance and its adaptability. How do we get that way? How do we move from the nearly helpless state of infants to the cognitive proficiency that characterizes adults? In this paper I argue, first, that analogical ability is the key factor in our prodigious capacity, and, second, that possession of a symbol system is crucial to the full expression of analogical ability.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • Self‐Explanations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems.Michelene T. H. Chi, Miriam Bassok, Matthew W. Lewis, Peter Reimann & Robert Glaser - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (2):145-182.
    The present paper analyzes the self‐generated explanations (from talk‐aloud protocols) that “Good” and “Poor” students produce while studying worked‐out examples of mechanics problems, and their subsequent reliance on examples during problem solving. We find that “Good” students learn with understanding: They generate many explanations which refine and expand the conditions for the action parts of the example solutions, and relate these actions to principles in the text. These self‐explanations are guided by accurate monitoring of their own understanding and misunderstanding. Such (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   84 citations  
  • Observing Tutorial Dialogues Collaboratively: Insights About Human Tutoring Effectiveness From Vicarious Learning.Michelene T. H. Chi, Marguerite Roy & Robert G. M. Hausmann - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (2):301-341.
    The goals of this study are to evaluate a relatively novel learning environment, as well as to seek greater understanding of why human tutoring is so effective. This alternative learning environment consists of pairs of students collaboratively observing a videotape of another student being tutored. Comparing this collaboratively observing environment to four other instructional methods—one‐on‐one human tutoring, observing tutoring individually, collaborating without observing, and studying alone—the results showed that students learned to solve physics problems just as effectively from observing tutoring (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Self‐Explanations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems.Michelene T. H. Chi, Miriam Bassok, Matthew W. Lewis, Peter Reimann & Robert Glaser - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (2):145-182.
    The present paper analyzes the self‐generated explanations (from talk‐aloud protocols) that “Good” and “Poor” students produce while studying worked‐out examples of mechanics problems, and their subsequent reliance on examples during problem solving. We find that “Good” students learn with understanding: They generate many explanations which refine and expand the conditions for the action parts of the example solutions, and relate these actions to principles in the text. These self‐explanations are guided by accurate monitoring of their own understanding and misunderstanding. Such (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • A Test of the Testing Effect: Acquiring Problem‐Solving Skills From Worked Examples.Tamara van Gog & Liesbeth Kester - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (8):1532-1541.
    The “testing effect” refers to the finding that after an initial study opportunity, testing is more effective for long‐term retention than restudying. The testing effect seems robust and is a finding from the field of cognitive science that has important implications for education. However, it is unclear whether this effect also applies to the acquisition of problem‐solving skills, which is important to establish given the key role problem solving plays in, for instance, math and science education. Worked examples are an (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Analogy Events: How Examples are Used During Problem Solving.Kurt VanLehn - 1998 - Cognitive Science 22 (3):347-388.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Worked Examples and Tutored Problem Solving: Redundant or Synergistic Forms of Support?Ron J. C. M. Salden, Vincent Awmm Aleven, Alexander Renkl & Rolf Schwonke - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (1):203-213.
    The current research investigates a combination of two instructional approaches, tutored problem solving and worked examples. Tutored problem solving with automated tutors has proven to be an effective instructional method. Worked‐out examples have been shown to be an effective complement to untutored problem solving, but it is largely unknown whether they are an effective complement to tutored problem solving. Further, while computer‐based learning environments offer the possibility of adaptively transitioning from examples to problems while tailoring to an individual learner, the (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Learning from worked‐out examples: A study on individual differences.Alexander Renkl - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (1):1-29.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Learning from Worked-Out Examples: A Study on Individual Differences.Alexander Renkl - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (1):1-29.
    The goal of this study was to investigate interindividual differences in learning from worked-out examples with respect to the quality of self-explanations. Restrictions of former studies (e.g., lacking control of time-on-task) were avoided and additional research questions (e.g., reliability and dimensionality of self-explanation characteristics) were addressed. An investigation with 36 university freshmen of education working in individual sessions was conducted. The domain was probability calculus. As predictors of learning, prior knowledge and the quality of self-explanations (thinking aloud protocols) were assessed. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • The Knowledge-Learning-Instruction Framework: Bridging the Science-Practice Chasm to Enhance Robust Student Learning.Kenneth R. Koedinger, Albert T. Corbett & Charles Perfetti - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (5):757-798.
    Despite the accumulation of substantial cognitive science research relevant to education, there remains confusion and controversy in the application of research to educational practice. In support of a more systematic approach, we describe the Knowledge-Learning-Instruction (KLI) framework. KLI promotes the emergence of instructional principles of high potential for generality, while explicitly identifying constraints of and opportunities for detailed analysis of the knowledge students may acquire in courses. Drawing on research across domains of science, math, and language learning, we illustrate the (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science.Dedre Gentner - 2003 - Nature Publishing Group.
  • The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning.K. Holyoak & B. Morrison (eds.) - 2005 - Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning is the first comprehensive and authoritative handbook covering all the core topics of the field of thinking and ...
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Complex declarative learning.Michelene Th Chi & Stellan Ohlsson - 2005 - In K. Holyoak & B. Morrison (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Cambridge University Press.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The analogical mind.Keith J. Holyoak & P. Thagard - 1997 - American Psychologist 52:35-44.
    We examine the use of analogy in human thinking from the perspective of a multiconstraint theory, which postulates three basic types of constraints: similarity, structure and purpose. The operation of these constraints is apparent in both laboratory experiments on analogy and in naturalistic settings, including politics, psychotherapy, and scientific research. We sketch how the multiconstraint theory can be implemented in detailed computational simulations of the analogical human mind.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Conditions and effects of example elaboration.Robin Stark, Heinz Mandl, Hans Gruber & Alexander Renkl - unknown
    The re-analysis aimed at extending earlier findings on example-based learning and to draw consequences for further research and instructional practice. Based on an earlier experimental study on learning with worked-out examples in the domain of accounting (n=56 students of a vocational school), we re-analysed the effects of an intervention means (elaboration training) on learning behaviour (aspects of example elaboration). In a further step, different ways of dealing with worked-out examples (elaboration pro-files) were identified and related to the subsequent learning outcomes (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning from worked-out examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations.Robin Stark, Alexander Renkl, Hans Gruber & Heinz Mandl - unknown
    It was investigated to what extent example variability and the elicitation of sophisticated self-explanations foster the acquisition of applicable and transferable knowledge by learning from worked-out examples. To this end, we had 56 apprentices from a bank learn calculation of compound interest and real interest. The subjects were randomly assigned to the four conditions of a 2´2-factorial design (factor 1: uniform vs. multiple examples; factor 2: spontaneous vs. Elicited self-explanations). The learning results were measured by a post-test comprising application problems (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations