Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Boredom: Under-aroused and restless.James Danckert, Tina Hammerschmidt, Jeremy Marty-Dugas & Daniel Smilek - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 61:24-37.
  • Anxiety and Attentional Bias: State and Trait.Donald Broadbent & Margaret Broadbent - 1988 - Cognition and Emotion 2 (3):165-183.
  • Boring thoughts and bored minds: The MAC model of boredom and cognitive engagement.Erin C. Westgate & Timothy D. Wilson - 2018 - Psychological Review 125 (5):689-713.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • A self-regulatory approach to understanding boredom proneness.A. A. Struk, A. A. Scholer & J. Danckert - 2016 - Cognition and Emotion 30 (8).
  • A Failure to Launch: Regulatory Modes and Boredom Proneness.Jhotisha Mugon, Andriy Struk & James Danckert - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
  • Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice.Jennifer S. Lerner & Dacher Keltner - 2000 - Cognition and Emotion 14 (4):473-493.
    Most theories of affective influences on judgement and choice take a valence-based approach, contrasting the effects of positive versus negative feeling states. These approaches have not specified if and when distinct emotions of the same valence have different effects on judgement. In this article, we propose a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. We posit that each emotion is defined by a tendency to perceive new events and objects in ways that are consistent with the original cognitive-appraisal dimensions (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   161 citations  
  • Strength of affective reaction as a signal to think carefully.Heather C. Lench & Shane W. Bench - 2015 - Cognition and Emotion 29 (2):220-235.
  • Effects of fear on risk and control judgements and memory: Implications for health promotion messages.Heather Lench & Linda Levine - 2005 - Cognition and Emotion 19 (7):1049-1069.
    Health promotion messages that evoke fear are often used to decrease unrealistic optimism regarding risks, convince people to control their behaviour, and make risks memorable. The relations among emotions, risk and control judgements, and memory are not well understood, however. In the current study, participants (N = 94) were assigned to fearful, angry, happy, or neutral emotion-elicitation conditions. They then rated the likelihood of experiencing 15 negative and 15 positive matched outcomes and rated their degree of control over each outcome. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Distraction from emotional information reduces biased judgements.Heather C. Lench, Shane W. Bench & Elizabeth L. Davis - 2016 - Cognition and Emotion 30 (4).
  • A Functionalist Manifesto: Goal-Related Emotions From an Evolutionary Perspective.Heather C. Lench, Shane W. Bench, Kathleen E. Darbor & Melody Moore - 2015 - Emotion Review 7 (1):90-98.
    Functional theories posit that emotions are elicited by particular goal-related situations that represented adaptive problems and that emotions are evolved features of coordinated responses to those situations. Yet little theory or research has addressed the evolutionary aspects of these theories. We apply five criteria that can be used to judge whether features are adaptations. There is evidence that sadness, anger, and anxiety relate to unique changes in physiology, cognition, and behavior, those changes are correlated, situations that give rise to emotions (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Emotion elicitation using films.James J. Gross & Robert W. Levenson - 1995 - Cognition and Emotion 9 (1):87-108.
  • Emotion, cognitive structure, and action tendency.Nico H. Frijda - 1987 - Cognition and Emotion 1 (2):115-143.
  • The Bright Side of Boredom.Andreas Elpidorou - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    The essay argues that boredom is an affective state that monitors and regulates our behavior. Boredom informs us when we are out of tune with our interests and motivates us to engage in situations that are perceived by us as fulfilling or meaningful. Boredom is thus important. It promotes our interests by trying to keep us in touch with what we care about. And it safeguards us from emotional traps and long-term dullness. -/- .
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Is boredom one or many? A functional solution to the problem of heterogeneity.Andreas Elpidorou - 2020 - Mind and Language 36 (3):491-511.
    Despite great progress in our theoretical and empirical investigations of boredom, a basic issue regarding boredom remains unresolved: it is still unclear whether the construct of boredom is a unitary one or not. By surveying the relevant literature on boredom and arousal, the paper makes a case for the unity of the construct of boredom. It argues, first, that extant empirical findings do not support the heterogeneity of boredom, and, second, that a theoretically motivated and empirically grounded model of boredom (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations