Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Toward a Science of Other Minds: Escaping the Argument by Analogy.Daniel J. Povinelli, Jesse M. Bering & Steve Giambrone - 2000 - Cognitive Science 24 (3):509-541.
    Since Darwin, the idea of psychological continuity between humans and other animals has dominated theory and research in investigating the minds of other species. Indeed, the field of comparative psychology was founded on two assumptions. First, it was assumed that introspection could provide humans with reliable knowledge about the causal connection between specific mental states and specific behaviors. Second, it was assumed that in those cases in which other species exhibited behaviors similar to our own, similar psychological causes were at (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Ontogeny, evolution, and folk psychology.Daniel J. Povinelli, Mia C. Zebouni & Christopher G. Prince - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):137-138.
    Barresi & Moore assume an equivalence between ontogenetic and evolutionaiy transformations of social understanding. The mechanisms of evolution allow for novel structures to arise, both through terminal addition and through the onset of novel pathways at time points that precede the end points of ancestral pathways. Terminal addition may not be the appropriate model for the evolution of human object-directed imitation, intermodal equivalence, or joint attention.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can quantum probability provide a new direction for cognitive modeling?Emmanuel M. Pothos & Jerome R. Busemeyer - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (3):255-274.
    Classical (Bayesian) probability (CP) theory has led to an influential research tradition for modeling cognitive processes. Cognitive scientists have been trained to work with CP principles for so long that it is hard even to imagine alternative ways to formalize probabilities. However, in physics, quantum probability (QP) theory has been the dominant probabilistic approach for nearly 100 years. Could QP theory provide us with any advantages in cognitive modeling as well? Note first that both CP and QP theory share the (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Hunting memes.H. C. Plotkin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):768-769.
  • Which came first, the egg-problem or the hen-solution?Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):84-86.
  • Progress toward an understanding of cortical computation.W. A. Phillips & W. Singer - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):703-714.
    The additional data, perspectives, questions, and criticisms contributed by the commentaries strengthen our view that local cortical processors coordinate their activity with the context in which it occurs using contextual fields and synchronized population codes. We therefore predict that whereas the specialization of function has been the keynote of this century the coordination of function will be the keynote of the next.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • In search of common foundations for cortical computation.William A. Phillips & Wolf Singer - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):657-683.
    It is worthwhile to search for forms of coding, processing, and learning common to various cortical regions and cognitive functions. Local cortical processors may coordinate their activity by maximizing the transmission of information coherently related to the context in which it occurs, thus forming synchronized population codes. This coordination involves contextual field (CF) connections that link processors within and between cortical regions. The effects of CF connections are distinguished from those mediating receptive field (RF) input; it is shown how CFs (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Tool use in birds: An avian monkey wrench?Irene M. Pepperberg - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):604-605.
  • The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later.Irene M. Pepperberg - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Imitation and derivative reactions.Sue Taylor Parker - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):604-604.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Social relations and understanding the intentional self.Annerieke Oosterwegel - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):136-136.
    Although Barresi & Moore could have grounded their framework more explicitly in existing models, they offer a provocative testbed for the assumptions of symbolic interactionism and further thinking about self-regulation, especially in autistics.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Understanding that looking causes knowing.David R. Olson & Bruce Homer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):135-135.
    Barresi & Moore provide an impressive account of how the coordination of first and third person information about the self and other could produce an account of intentional relations. They are less explicit as to how the child comes to understand the basic epistemic relation between experience and knowledge, that is, how informational access causes belief. We suggest one route.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biotic intelligence (BI)?F. J. Odling-Smee - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):83-84.
  • Evolving remembrance of times past and future.William Noble & Iain Davidson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):572-572.
  • Solving the “human problem”: The frontal feedback model.Raymond A. Noack - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (2):1043-1067.
    This paper argues that humans possess unique cognitive abilities due to the presence of a functional system that exists in the human brain that is absent in the non-human brain. This system, the frontal feedback system, was born in the hominin brain when the great phylogenetic expansion of the prefrontal cortex relative to posterior sensory regions surpassed a critical threshold. Surpassing that threshold effectively reversed the preferred direction of information flow in the highest association regions of the neocortex, producing the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Absolute Numerosity Discrimination as a Case Study in Comparative Vertebrate Intelligence.Andreas Nieder - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Four-year-old humans are different: Why?Katherine Nelson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):134-135.
    The intentionality schema is an abstraction that relates phylogenetic and ontogenetic sequences of social understanding, but it also obscures the differences between humans and other primates. In particular, it ignores human social developmental and communicative history and the important roles that language plays in human understanding of others' intentional states.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • But what is the intentional schema?Adam Morton - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):133-134.
    The intentional schema may not be sufficiently characterized to make questions about its role in individual and species development intelligible. The idea of metarepresentation may perhaps give it enough content. The importance of metarepresentation itself, however, can be called into question.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self-knowledge, knowledge of other minds, and kinesthetic-visual matching.Robert W. Mitchell - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):133-133.
    The “intentional schema” seems identical to or dependent upon kinesthetic–visual matching, both of which account for similar empirical findings. The intentional schema, however, fails to account for variability in children's understanding of false belief and differences in children's understanding of self and other in pretense.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Apes have mimetic culture.Robert W. Mitchell & H. Lyn Miles - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):768-768.
  • Is intelligent behavior a directly observable phenomenon?E. W. Menzel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):603-604.
  • Nesting cups and metatools in chimpanzees.Tetsuro Matsuzawa - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):570-571.
  • Adaptive rationality and identifiability of psychological processes.Dominic W. Massaro & Daniel Friedman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):499-501.
  • Correct data base: Wrong model?Alexander Marshack - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):767-768.
  • The way of all matter.William A. MacKay - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):82-83.
  • Tool use implies sensorimotor skill: But differences in skills do not imply differences in intelligence.Euan M. Macphail - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):602-603.
  • Linguistic and manual evolution.Peter F. MacNeilage - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):568-570.
  • Comparative psychology: New experimental findings, not new approaches, are needed.Euan M. Macphail - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):395-398.
  • Lessons from evolution for artificial intelligence?Rudi Lutz - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):766-766.
  • G and g: Two markers of a general cognitive ability, or none?Charles Locurto - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Intelligent” evolution and neo-Darwinian straw men.Elisabeth A. Lloyd - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):81-82.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Speech and brain evolution.Philip Lieberman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):566-568.
  • Evolution, development, and learning in cognitive science.David Leiser - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):80-81.
  • Language equals mimesis plus speech.Aarre Laakso - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):765-766.
  • The applicability of Piagetian concepts to animals.Adriaan Kortlandt - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):601-601.
  • The gradual evolution of enhanced control by plans: A view from below.Leonard D. Katz - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):764-765.
  • Constructivism without tears.Annette Karmiloff-Smith & Mark H. Johnson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):566-566.
  • Active symbols and internal models: Towards a cognitive connectionism. [REVIEW]Stephen Kaplan, Mark Weaver & Robert French - 1990 - AI and Society 4 (1):51-71.
    In the first section of the article, we examine some recent criticisms of the connectionist enterprise: first, that connectionist models are fundamentally behaviorist in nature (and, therefore, non-cognitive), and second that connectionist models are fundamentally associationist in nature (and, therefore, cognitively weak). We argue that, for a limited class of connectionist models (feed-forward, pattern-associator models), the first criticism is unavoidable. With respect to the second criticism, we propose that connectionist modelsare fundamentally associationist but that this is appropriate for building models (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   81 citations  
  • Species intelligence: Analogy without homology.James W. Kalat - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):80-80.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The right tools for the job?Mark Johnson & Annette Karmiloff-Smith - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):600-600.
  • Similarities and dissimilarities between adaptation and learning.Mark H. Johnson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):79-80.
  • Piagetian stages and the anagenetic study of cognitive evolution.Timothy D. Johnston - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):600-601.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolved mind.Harry J. Jerison - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):763-764.
  • Neurobiology and language acquisition: Continuity and identity.Bob Jacobs - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):565-565.
  • Probing the “Achilles' heel” of rational analysis.Keith J. Holyoak - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):498-499.
  • Understanding minds and selves.R. Peter Hobson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):132-132.
    Barresi & Moore provide a welcome focus on children's abilities to integrate first and third person information about intentional relations but they pay insufficient attention to the origins of children's understanding of the nature of subjective orientations vis-à-vis a shared world and the potential significance of such understanding as a source (rather than an outcome) of domain-general information-processing capacities.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Imagination and imitation: Input, acid test, or alchemy?C. M. Heyes - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):131-132.
    Immediate imitation is likely to be a major, direct input to Barresi & Moore's level 2 competence, but deferred imitation is unlikely to play a key role in the transition to level 3, because (1) the attribution of first person knowledge is neither a necessary cause nor an obvious consequence of deferred imitation, and (2) deferred imitation does not correlate phylogenetically with capacities that more plausibly either yield or reflect a concept of intentional agency.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Species intelligence: Hazards of structural parallels.Robert W. Hendersen - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):78-79.
  • If you've got it, why not flaunt it? Monkeys with Broca's area but no syntactical structure to their vocal utterances.Marc D. Hauser - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):564-564.
  • Rational analysis and the Lens model.Reid Hastie & Kenneth R. Hammond - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):498-498.