Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Attention: some theoretical considerations.J. A. Deutsch & D. Deutsch - 1963 - Psychological Review 70 (1):80-90.
    The selection of wanted from unwanted messages requires discriminatory mechanisms of as great a complexity as those in normal perception, as is indicated by behavioral evidence. The results of neurophysiology experiments on selective attention are compatible with this supposition. This presents a difficulty for Filter theory. Another mechanism is proposed, which assumes the existence of a shifting reference standard, which takes up the level of the most important arriving signal. The way such importance is determined in the system is further (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   159 citations  
  • Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Wisdom, but not especially unconventional.Robert G. Crowder - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):72-72.
  • Modular mind or unitary system: A duck-rabbit effect.Gillian Cohen - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):71-72.
  • The Maltese cross: Simplistic yes, new no.Thomas H. Carr & Tracy L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):69-71.
  • Models of mind: Hidden plumbing.Enoch Callaway - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):68-69.
  • The Maltese cross: A new simplistic model for memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):55-68.
    This paper puts forward a general framework for thought about human information processing. It is intended to avoid some of the problems of pipeline or stage models of function. At the same time it avoids the snare of supposing a welter of indefinitely many separate processes. The approach is not particularly original, but rather represents the common elements or presuppositions in a number of modern theories. These presuppositions are not usually explicit, however, and making them so reduces the danger of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   58 citations  
  • Modules in models of memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-94.
    This paper puts forward a general framework for thought about human information processing. It is intended to avoid some of the problems of pipeline or stage models of function. At the same time it avoids the snare of supposing a welter of indefinitely many separate processes. The approach is not particularly original, but rather represents the common elements or presuppositions in a number of modern theories. These presuppositions are not usually explicit, however, and making them so reduces the danger of (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connecting cognition and consumer choice.Daniel M. Bartels & Eric J. Johnson - 2015 - Cognition 135:47-51.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Models as toothbrushes.Michael J. Watkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-86.
  • How do representations get processed in real nerve cells?Gerald S. Wasserman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):85-85.
  • Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method.Saul Sternberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):82-84.
  • Pipelines, processing models, and the mindbody problem.John G. Seamon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):81-82.
  • Practice, attention, and the processing system.Walter Schneider - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):80-81.
  • Information-flow diagrams as scientific models.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):79-80.
  • The use of interference paradigms as a criterion for separating memory stores.Henry L. Roediger - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):78-79.
  • Simplistic heuristics and Maltese acrostics.Patrick Rabbitt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):77-78.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The usefulness for memory theory of the word “store”.D. J. Murray - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):76-77.
  • What kind of a framework?John Morton - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-76.
  • Memory and mood.Maryanne Martin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-75.
  • The homunculus as bureaucrat.Alan K. Mackworth - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):74-74.
  • Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Something old, something new, something borrowed, something missing.Elizabeth F. Loftus, Geoffrey R. Loftus & Earl B. Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):73-74.
  • Perceptual Restoration of Temporally Distorted Speech in L1 vs. L2: Local Time Reversal and Modulation Filtering.Mako Ishida, Takayuki Arai & Makio Kashino - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Speech is intelligible even when the temporal envelope of speech is distorted. The current study investigates how native and non-native speakers perceptually restore temporally distorted speech. Participants were native English speakers (NS), and native Japanese speakers who spoke English as a second language (NNS). In Experiment 1, participants listened to “locally time-reversed speech” where every x-ms of speech signal was reversed on the temporal axis. Here, the local time reversal shifted the constituents of the speech signal forward or backward from (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Practice and divided attention.William Hirst - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):72-73.
  • Laterality in Emotional Language Processing in First and Second Language.Raheleh Heyrani, Vahid Nejati, Sara Abbasi & Gesa Hartwigsen - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Language is a cognitive function that is asymmetrically distributed across both hemispheres, with left dominance for most linguistic operations. One key question of interest in cognitive neuroscience studies is related to the contribution of both hemispheres in bilingualism. Previous work shows a difference of both hemispheres for auditory processing of emotional and non-emotional words in bilinguals and monolinguals. In this study, we examined the differences between both hemispheres in the processing of emotional and non-emotional words of mother tongue language and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensation's ghost: The nonsensory fringe of consciousness.Bruce Mangan - 2001 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 7.
    Non-sensory experiences represent almost all context information in consciousness. They condition most aspects of conscious cognition including voluntary retrieval, perception, monitoring, problem solving, emotion, evaluation, meaning recognition. Many peculiar aspects of non-sensory qualia (e.g., they resist being 'grasped' by an act of attention) are explained as adaptations shaped by the cognitive functions they serve. The most important nonsensory experience is coherence or "rightness." Rightness represents degrees of context fit among contents in consciousness, and between conscious and non-conscious processes. Rightness (not (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations