Results for 'T. Baars'

988 found
Order:
  1.  12
    Semantic bias effects on the outcomes of verbal slips.Michael T. Motley & Bernard J. Baars - 1976 - Cognition 4 (2):177-187.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  2.  12
    Reliability and Validity of the Utrecht Tasks for Attention in Toddlers Using Eye Tracking.Anneloes L. van Baar, Marjanneke de Jong, Martine Maat, Ignace T. C. Hooge, Lilly Bogičević & Marjolein Verhoeven - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3. Peer Commentary on ‘Are There Neural Correlates of Consciousness?’.B. Baars, T. Bayne, W. Freeman & V. Hardcastle - 2004 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 11 (1):29-86.
  4.  41
    Comte, x Coombs, CH, 31, 36 Cox. LE, 205,207 Darwin, C., 29, 36.R. Abelson, L. Addis, K. D. Allen, W. P. Alston, J. T. Andresen, D. M. Armstrong, W. J. Arnold, K. J. Arrow, B. J. Baars & A. Bandura - 1999 - In Bruce A. Thyer (ed.), The Philosophical Legacy of Behaviorism. Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 257.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  28
    Introduction of the Utrecht Tasks for Attention in Toddlers Using Eye Tracking : A Pilot Study.Marjanneke de Jong, Marjolein Verhoeven, Ignace T. C. Hooge & Anneloes L. van Baar - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6. The Role of the Concept of Natural (Naturalness) in Organic Fanning.H. Veerhog, M. Mat2e, E. Lammerts van Bueren & T. Baars - 2003 - Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 16:29-49.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  7. Subjective experience is probably not limited to humans: The evidence from neurobiology and behavior.Bernard J. Baars - 2005 - Consciousness and Cognition 14 (1):7-21.
    In humans, conscious perception and cognition depends upon the thalamocortical complex, which supports perception, explicit cognition, memory, language, planning, and strategic control. When parts of the T-C system are damaged or stimulated, corresponding effects are found on conscious contents and state, as assessed by reliable reports. In contrast, large regions like cerebellum and basal ganglia can be damaged without affecting conscious cognition directly. Functional brain recordings also show robust activity differences in cortex between experimentally matched conscious and unconscious events. This (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  8. On Baars' psychologization of Skinnerism.T. R. Machan - 2003 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 10 (1):54-58.
  9. Reply to Baars.Teed Rockwell - unknown
    My claim that Skinner believed in psychological atoms is actually strengthened by Baars' remark that Skinner 's behaviorist atoms could take a variety of physical forms. Baars is correct that Pavlov, unlike Skinner, thought that psychological atoms were identical to certain physiological items. But Skinner, as a non-reductive atomist, thought he could permit his psychological atoms to have a variety of physical forms. He still believed that even though each S-R connection was not really physical, it could nevertheless (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  21
    Why dont we know what Mary knows? Baars' reversing the problem of qualia.A. C. Elitzur - 1997 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 4 (4):319.
    Baars’ Global Workspace theory suggests that consciousness functions as a gateway, facilitating focused access to any part of the brain. While this hypothesis does not address the ‘hard problems’, namely, the very nature of consciousness, it constrains any theory that attempts to do so and provides important insights into the relation between consciousness and cognition. Many questions have found new answers once they were turned upside down. In medicine, for example, important discoveries have been made when, instead of asking (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11. Consciousness is computational: The Lida model of global workspace theory.Bernard J. Baars & Stan Franklin - 2009 - International Journal of Machine Consciousness 1 (1):23-32.
    The currently leading cognitive theory of consciousness, Global Workspace Theory,1,2 postulates that the primary functions of consciousness include a global broadcast serving to recruit internal resources with which to deal with the current situation and to modulate several types of learning. In addition, conscious experiences present current conditions and problems to a "self" system, an executive interpreter that is identifiable with brain structures like the frontal lobes and precuneus.1Be it human, animal or artificial, an autonomous agent3 is said to be (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  12. In the Theater of Consciousness: The Workspace of the Mind.Bernard J. Baars - 1997 - New York: Oxford University Press USA.
    The study of conscious experience has seen remarkable strides in the last ten years, reflecting important technological breakthroughs and the enormous efforts of researchers in disciplines as varied as neuroscience, cognitive science, and philosophy. Although still embroiled in debate, scientists are now beginning to find common ground in their understanding of consciousness, which may pave the way for a unified explanation of how and why we experience and understand the world around us. Written by eminent psychologist Bernard J. Baars, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  13. The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence.Bernard J. Baars - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (1):47-52.
  14. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness.Bernard J. Baars - 1988 - New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Conscious experience is one of the most difficult and thorny problems in psychological science. Its study has been neglected for many years, either because it was thought to be too difficult, or because the relevant evidence was thought to be poor. Bernard Baars suggests a way to specify empirical constraints on a theory of consciousness by contrasting well-established conscious phenomena - such as stimulus representations known to be attended, perceptual, and informative - with closely comparable unconscious ones - such (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   549 citations  
  15. How conscious experience and working memory interact.Bernard J. Baars & Stan Franklin - 2003 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (4):166-172.
  16.  48
    Global Workspace Dynamics: Cortical “Binding and Propagation” Enables Conscious Contents.Bernard J. Baars, Stan Franklin & Thomas Zoega Ramsoy - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
  17.  13
    On returning to consciousness.B. Baars - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (1):1-2.
  18.  11
    Pierre Poiret : le plus fidèle disciple d’Antoinette Bourignon.Mirjam de Baar - 2021 - Revue de Théologie Et de Philosophie 153 (1):19-35.
    C’est en grande partie grâce à sa connaissance de l’œuvre de la prophétesse Antoinette Bourignon (1616-1680) que le cartésien Pierre Poiret est devenu un fervent défenseur de la mystique. Après leur rencontre en 1676, Poiret devint son disciple le plus dévoué et l’éditeur en chef de ses écrits. Cependant, à l’époque de leur première rencontre il n’était pas du tout prévu qu’il assume ce rôle. En fait, de nouvelles recherches montrent que pendant deux ans, Bourignon a continué à mettre Poiret (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  6
    In the Theater of Consciousness: The Workspace of the Mind.Bernard J. Baars - 1997 - New York: Oxford University Press USA.
    Written by eminent psychologist Bernard J. Baars, this book brings us to the frontlines of the consciousness debate, offering the general reader a fascinating overview of how top scientists currently understand the processes underlying conscious experience. The study of conscious experience has seen remarkable strides in the last ten years, reflecting important technological breakthroughs and the enormous efforts of researchers in disciplines as varied as neuroscience, cognitive science, and philosophy. Scientists are just now beginning to find common ground in (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  20.  19
    Divided consciousness or divided self?B. Baars - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (1):59-60.
  21.  37
    Introduction: The evidence for anosognosia.B. Baars - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (2):148-151.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22. The functions of consciousness.Bernard J. Baars - 1988 - In A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  23.  68
    Surprisingly small subcortical structures are needed for the state of waking consciousness, while cortical projection areas seem to provide perceptual contents of consciousness.Bernard J. Baars - 1995 - Consciousness and Cognition 4 (2):159-62.
  24.  65
    Does Philosophy Help or Hinder Scientific Work on Consciousness?Bernard J. Baars & Katharine McGovern - 1993 - Consciousness and Cognition 2 (1):18-27.
  25.  34
    The Global Workspace Theory of Consciousness.Bernard J. Baars - 2017 - In Susan Schneider & Max Velmans (eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness. Chichester, UK: Wiley. pp. 227–242.
    Global Workspace Theory (GWT) can be compared to a theater of mind, in which conscious contents resemble a bright spot on the stage of immediate memory, selected by a spotlight of attention under executive guidance. Only the bright spot is conscious; the rest of the theater is dark and unconscious. GWT has been implemented in a number of explicit and testable global workspace models (GWM's). These specific GW models suggest that conscious experiences recruit widely distributed brain functions that are mostly (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  26. A communication algorithm for teamwork in multi-agent environments.Egon van Baars & Rineke Verbrugge - 2009 - Journal of Applied Non-Classical Logics 19 (4):431-461.
    Using a knowledge-based approach, we derive a protocol, MACOM1, for the sequence transmission problem from one agent to a group of agents. The protocol is correct for communication media where deletion and reordering errors may occur. Furthermore, it is shown that after k rounds the agents in the group attain depth k general knowledge about the members of the group and the values of the messages. Then, we adjust this algorithm for multi-agent communication for the process of teamwork. MACOM1 solves (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27. The role of consciousness in memory.S. Franklin, B. J. Baars, U. Ramamurthy & M. Ventura - 2005 - Brains, Minds and Media 1.
    Conscious events interact with memory systems in learning, rehearsal and retrieval (Ebbinghaus 1885/1964; Tulving 1985). Here we present hypotheses that arise from the IDA computional model (Franklin,Kelemen and McCauley 1998; Franklin 2001b) of global workspace theory (Baars 1988, 2002). Our primary tool for this exploration is a flexible cognitive cycle employed by the IDA computational model and hypothesized to be a basic element of human cognitive processing. Since cognitive cycles are hypothesized to occur five to tentimes a second and (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  28. Some essential differences between consciousness and attention, perception, and working memory.Bernard J. Baars - 1997 - Consciousness and Cognition 6 (2-3):363-371.
    When “divided attention” methods were discovered in the 1950s their implications for conscious experience were not widely appreciated. Yet when people process competing streams of sensory input they show both selective processesandclear contrasts between conscious and unconscious events. This paper suggests that the term “attention” may be best applied to theselection and maintenanceof conscious contents and distinguished from consciousness itself. This is consistent with common usage. The operational criteria for selective attention, defined in this way, are entirely different from those (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  29.  24
    Putting the focus on the fringe: Three empirical cases.Bernard J. Baars - 1993 - Consciousness and Cognition 2 (2):126-36.
    After suggesting an operational definition for fringe experiences—as opposed to clearly conscious and clearly unconscious phenomena—we examine three empirical cases: The tip-of-the-tongue experience, the fringe experience of "wrongness," and the case of conscious focus on abstract, hard-to-image conscious contents. In each case, Mangan′s four major claims are explored in some detail. Most tasks seem to involve a combination of conscious experiences, complex unconscious representations, and multiple fringe experiences. The chief disagreement from this analysis involves vague experiences that are generally believed (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  30.  37
    Why volition is a foundation issue for psychology.Bernard J. Baars - 1993 - Consciousness and Cognition 2 (4):281-309.
    Since the advent of behaviorism the question of volition or "will" has been largely neglected. We consider evidence indicating that two identical behaviors may be quite distinct with respect to volition: For instance, with practice the details of predictable actions become less and less voluntary, even if the behavior itself does not visibly change. Likewise, people can voluntarily imitate involuntary slips they have just made. Such examples suggest that the concept of volition applies not to visible behavior per se, but (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  31.  10
    Putting the Focus on the Fringe: Three Empirical Cases.Bernard J. Baars - 1993 - Consciousness and Cognition 2 (2):126-136.
    After suggesting an operational definition for fringe experiences—as opposed to clearly conscious and clearly unconscious phenomena—we examine three empirical cases: The tip-of-the-tongue experience, the fringe experience of "wrongness," and the case of conscious focus on abstract, hard-to-image conscious contents. In each case, Mangan′s four major claims are explored in some detail. Most tasks seem to involve a combination of conscious experiences, complex unconscious representations, and multiple fringe experiences. The chief disagreement from this analysis involves vague experiences that are generally believed (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  32. In the theatre of consciousness: Global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness.Bernard J. Baars - 1997 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 4 (4):292-309.
    Can we make progress exploring consciousness? Or is it forever beyond human reach? In science we never know the ultimate outcome of the journey. We can only take whatever steps our current knowledge affords. This paper explores today's evidence from the viewpoint of Global Workspace theory. First, we ask what kind of evidence has the most direct bearing on the question. The answer given here is ‘contrastive analysis’ -- a set of paired comparisons between similar conscious and unconscious processes. This (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   133 citations  
  33.  44
    The Effectiveness of Art Therapy for Anxiety in Adult Women: A Randomized Controlled Trial.Annemarie Abbing, Erik W. Baars, Leo de Sonneville, Anne S. Ponstein & Hanna Swaab - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
  34.  37
    Self-report measures of executive functioning are a determinant of academic performance in first-year students at a university of applied sciences.Maria A. E. Baars, Marije Nije Bijvank, Geertje H. Tonnaer & Jelle Jolles - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  35.  33
    A curious coincidence? Consciousness as an object of scientific scrutiny fits our personal experience remarkably well.Bernard J. Baars - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):669-670.
  36.  5
    Exploring Students’ Use of a Mobile Application to Support Their Self-Regulated Learning Processes.Martine Baars, Sanyogita Khare & Léonie Ridderstap - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Being able to self-regulate one’s learning is essential for academic success but is also very difficult for students. Especially first year students can be overwhelmed with the high study load and autonomy in higher education. To face this challenge, students’ monitoring and self-regulated learning processes are crucial. Yet, often students are not aware of effective SRL strategies or how to use them. In this study, the use of a mobile application with gamification elements to support first-year university students’ SRL processes (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  37.  15
    A psychologically implausible architecture that is always conscious, always active.Mark Vincent LaPolla & Bernard J. Baars - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):448-449.
  38. Global workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience?Bernard J. Baars - 2006 - In Steven Laureys (ed.), Boundaries of Consciousness. Elsevier.
  39. Neural darwinism and consciousness.Anil K. Seth & Bernard J. Baars - 2005 - Consciousness and Cognition 14 (1):140-168.
    Neural Darwinism (ND) is a large scale selectionist theory of brain development and function that has been hypothesized to relate to consciousness. According to ND, consciousness is entailed by reentrant interactions among neuronal populations in the thalamocortical system (the ‘dynamic core’). These interactions, which permit high-order discriminations among possible core states, confer selective advantages on organisms possessing them by linking current perceptual events to a past history of value-dependent learning. Here, we assess the consistency of ND with 16 widely recognized (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  40. Applying global workspace theory to the frame problem.Murray Shanahan & Bernard Baars - 2005 - Cognition 98 (2):157-176.
  41.  28
    Editorial: What can Neuroscience Learn from Contemplative Practices?Zoran Josipovic & Bernard J. Baars - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  42. One, not two, neural correlates of consciousness.Bernard J. Baars & Steven Laureys - 2005 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (6):269.
  43. Consciousness provides the nervous system with coherent, globally distributed information.B. J. Baars - 1983 - In Richard J. Davidson, Gary E. Schwartz & D. H. Shapiro (eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation. Plenum. pp. 101.
  44.  31
    Conscious contents provide the nervous system with coherent, global information.Bernard J. Baars - 1983 - In Richard J. Davidson, Gary E. Schwartz & D. H. Shapiro (eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation. Plenum. pp. 41--79.
  45. Brain, conscious experience, and the observing self.Bernard J. Baars, Thomas Zoega Ramsoy & Steven Laureys - 2003 - Trends in Neurosciences 26 (12):671-5.
    Conscious perception, like the sight of a coffee cup, seems to involve the brain identifying a stimulus. But conscious input activates more brain regions than are needed to identify coffee cups and faces. It spreads beyond sensory cortex to frontoparietal association areas, which do not serve stimulus identification as such. What is the role of those regions? Parietal cortex support the ‘first person perspective’ on the visual world, unconsciously framing the visual object stream. Some prefrontal areas select and interpret conscious (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   60 citations  
  46.  23
    The Association between Motivation, Affect, and Self-regulated Learning When Solving Problems.Baars Martine, Wijnia Lisette & Paas Fred - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  47.  63
    Consciousness eclipsed: Jacques Loeb, Ivan P. Pavlov, and the rise of reductionistic biology after 1900.Ralph J. Greenspan & Bernard J. Baars - 2005 - Consciousness and Cognition 14 (1):219-230.
    The life sciences in the 20th century were guided to a large extent by a reductionist program seeking to explain biological phenomena in terms of physics and chemistry. Two scientists who figured prominently in the establishment and dissemination of this program were Jacques Loeb in biology and Ivan P. Pavlov in psychological behaviorism. While neither succeeded in accounting for higher mental functions in physical-chemical terms, both adopted positions that reduced the problem of consciousness to the level of reflexes and associations. (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  48. Peer commentary on are there neural correlates of consciousness: A stew of confusion.Bernard J. Baars - 2004 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 11 (1):29-31.
  49. on 'Are There Neural Correlates of Consciousness?'.Bernard Baars - 2004 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 11 (1):29-86.
  50.  28
    Biological implications of a Global Workspace theory of consciousness: Evidence, theory, and some phylogenetic speculations.Bernard J. Baars - 1987 - In G. Greenberg & E. Tobach (eds.), Cognition, Language, and Consciousness: Integrative Levels. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 209--236.
1 — 50 / 988