About the Origins of the Human Ability to Create Constructs of Reality

Axiomathes 32 (6):1505-1524 (2022)
  Copy   BIBTEX

Abstract

The competence of humans to create and apply constructs of reality far exceeds that of any other animal species. Their ability to consciously manipulate such models seems unique, but it remains unknown how these abilities were initially acquired and then developed. Most individuals hold strong, culturally-anchored beliefs that their particular reality is true, a viewpoint challenged by the observation that all such constructs are different. They reflect not reality, but each individual’s life experiences. Collectively they facilitated the development of hominins to unprecedented cultural and cognitive complexity. However, it remains entirely unknown how the human brain manages to create a model of the external world from the signals provided by sensory equipment and proprioceptors. This paper examines the roles of exograms in this development, as they are considered to be the only tangible connection between the brain, the faculties of sentience and the external world. Competency in exogram use became a crucial natural selection factor for humans and even overcame the human brain atrophy of the final Pleistocene and the Holocene. Under favourable conditions, some forms of exograms are capable of surviving from the deep time of human evolution. The paper follows their trail back in time to gain some insights into the developments that gave rise to human awareness, self-consciousness and Theory of Mind as we understand them. Specific archaeological finds and notions about sentient capabilities of hominins are presented in a search for exogram use in the course of human evolution. It results in a model that explains with clarity not only the course of the human journey but also the underlying reasons for the human condition as such: why we are the way we are.

Links

PhilArchive



    Upload a copy of this work     Papers currently archived: 92,227

External links

Setup an account with your affiliations in order to access resources via your University's proxy server

Through your library

Similar books and articles

Del uso de la voluntad en el De anima de Suárez.Idoya Zorroza - 2017 - Scientia et Fides 5 (1):223-239.
The origins of causal cognition in early hominins.Martin Stuart-Fox - 2015 - Biology and Philosophy 30 (2):247-266.
The Subjective Brain, Identity, and Neuroethics.Grant R. Gillett - 2009 - American Journal of Bioethics 9 (9):5-13.
Cultural evolution is not equivalent to Darwinian evolution.Dwight W. Read - 2006 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29 (4):361-361.
Technologizing the human condition: hyperconnectivity and control.Trevor Thwaites - 2021 - Educational Philosophy and Theory 53 (4):373-382.
Social theory and human reality.Pertti Alasuutari - 2004 - Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage Publications.
Human tool behavior is species-specific and remains unique.Susan Cachel - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (4):222-222.
Simone Weil’s Radical Ontology of Rootedness: Natural and Supernatural Justices.Alexandre A. Martins - 2019 - Praxis: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Faith and Justice 2 (1):23-35.

Analytics

Added to PP
2021-02-16

Downloads
21 (#740,927)

6 months
7 (#438,648)

Historical graph of downloads
How can I increase my downloads?

Citations of this work

No citations found.

Add more citations

References found in this work

The extended mind.Andy Clark & David J. Chalmers - 1998 - Analysis 58 (1):7-19.
We have never been modern.Bruno Latour - 1993 - Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
The problems of philosophy.Bertrand Russell - 1912 - New York: Barnes & Noble.
The Bounds of Cognition.Frederick Adams & Kenneth Aizawa - 2008 - Malden, MA, USA: Wiley-Blackwell. Edited by Kenneth Aizawa.

View all 30 references / Add more references