Results for 'Visual Space Perception'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Space Perception, Visual Dissonance and the Fate of Standard Representationalism.Farid Masrour - 2017 - Noûs 51 (3):565-593.
    This paper argues that a common form of representationalism has trouble accommodating empirical findings about visual space perception. Vision science tells us that the visual system systematically gives rise to different experiences of the same spatial property. This, combined with a naturalistic account of content, suggests that the same spatial property can have different veridical looks. I use this to argue that a common form of representationalism about spatial experience must be rejected. I conclude by considering (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  2.  16
    Accommodation and convergence in visual space perception.V. W. Grant - 1942 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 31 (2):89.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  3.  32
    Visual space-perceptions in the dark.W. H. S. Monok - 1884 - Mind 9 (36):617-617.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4. Studies in Auditory and Visual Space Perception.Arthur Henry Pierce - 1902 - The Monist 12:476.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5. Studies in Auditory and Visual Space Perception.A. Pierce - 1902 - Revue Philosophique de la France Et de l'Etranger 54:189-194.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  22
    Studies in Auditory and Visual Space Perception.Charles H. Judd - 1902 - Philosophical Review 11 (3):303-307.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7. Philosophy of Perception and the Phenomenology of Visual Space.Gary Hatfield - 2011 - Philosophic Exchange 42 (1):31-66.
    In the philosophy of perception, direct realism has come into vogue. Philosophical authors assert and assume that what their readers want, and what anyone should want, is some form of direct realism. There are disagreements over precisely what form this direct realism should take. The majority of positions in favor now offer a direct realism in which objects and their material or physical properties constitute the contents of perception, either because we have an immediate or intuitive acquaintance with (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  8. The geometry of visual space and the nature of visual experience.Farid Masrour - 2015 - Philosophical Studies 172 (7):1813-1832.
    Some recently popular accounts of perception account for the phenomenal character of perceptual experience in terms of the qualities of objects. My concern in this paper is with naturalistic versions of such a phenomenal externalist view. Focusing on visual spatial perception, I argue that naturalistic phenomenal externalism conflicts with a number of scientific facts about the geometrical characteristics of visual spatial experience.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  9. Balint’s Syndrome, Visual Motion Perception, and Awareness of Space.Bartek Chomanski - 2018 - Erkenntnis 83 (6):1265-1284.
    Kant, Wittgenstein, and Husserl all held that visual awareness of objects requires visual awareness of the space in which the objects are located. There is a lively debate in the literature on spatial perception whether this view is undermined by the results of experiments on a Balint’s syndrome patient, known as RM. I argue that neither of two recent interpretations of these results is able to explain RM’s apparent ability to experience motion. I outline some ways (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. Representation and constraints: The inverse problem and the structure of visual space.Gary Hatfield - 2003 - Acta Psychologica 114:355-378.
    Visual space can be distinguished from physical space. The first is found in visual experience, while the second is defined independently of perception. Theorists have wondered about the relation between the two. Some investigators have concluded that visual space is non-Euclidean, and that it does not have a single metric structure. Here it is argued that visual space exhibits contraction in all three dimensions with increasing distance from the observer, that experienced (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  11.  16
    Review of Studies in Auditory and Visual Space Perception[REVIEW]James Rowland Angell - 1902 - Psychological Review 9 (4):397-401.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  42
    Visual space is not cognitively impenetrable.Yiannis Aloimonos & Cornelia Fermüller - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (3):366-367.
    Cognitive impenetrability (CI) of a large part of visual perception is taken for granted by those of us in the field of computational vision who attempt to recover descriptions of space using geometry and statistics as tools. These tools clearly point out, however, that CI cannot extend to the level of structured descriptions of object surfaces, as Pylyshyn suggests. The reason is that visual space – the description of the world inside our heads – is (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13. Visual space from the perspective of possible-worlds semantics, I.L. Wiesenthal - 1983 - Synthese 56 (August):199-238.
  14. Two visual systems and two theories of perception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological approaches.Joel Norman - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (1):73-96.
    The two contrasting theoretical approaches to visual perception, the constructivist and the ecological, are briefly presented and illustrated through their analyses of space and size perception. Earlier calls for their reconciliation and unification are reviewed. Neurophysiological, neuropsychological, and psychophysical evidence for the existence of two quite distinct visual systems, the ventral and the dorsal, is presented. These two perceptual systems differ in their functions; the ventral system's central function is that of identification, while the dorsal (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  15.  65
    Variations in the Anisotropy and Affine Structure of Visual Space: A Geometry of Visibles with a Third Dimension.Mark Wagner & Anthony J. Gambino - 2016 - Topoi 35 (2):583-598.
    A meta-analysis and an experiment show that the degree of compression of the in-depth dimension of visual space relative to the frontal dimension increases quickly as a function of the distance between the stimulus and the observer at first, but the rate of change slows beyond 7 m from the observer, reaching an apparent asymptote of about 50 %. In addition, the compression of visual space is greater for monocular and reduced cue conditions. The pattern of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  16.  49
    Synaesthetic perception of colour and visual space in a blind subject: An fMRI case study.Valentina Niccolai, Tessa M. van Leeuwen, Colin Blakemore & Petra Stoerig - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (2):889-899.
    In spatial sequence synaesthesia ordinal stimuli are perceived as arranged in peripersonal space. Using fMRI, we examined the neural bases of SSS and colour synaesthesia for spoken words in a late-blind synaesthete, JF. He reported days of the week and months of the year as both coloured and spatially ordered in peripersonal space; parts of the days and festivities of the year were spatially ordered but uncoloured. Words that denote time-units and triggered no concurrents were used in a (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  17.  22
    The quantized geometry of visual space: The coherent computation of depth, form, and lightness.Stephen Grossberg - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):625.
  18.  18
    Visual processing in three-dimensional space: Perceptions and misperceptions.Fred H. Previc - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):559-575.
  19.  24
    Perceptual load influences auditory space perception in the ventriloquist aftereffect.Ranmalee Eramudugolla, Marc R. Kamke, Salvador Soto-Faraco & Jason B. Mattingley - 2011 - Cognition 118 (1):62-74.
    A period of exposure to trains of simultaneous but spatially offset auditory and visual stimuli can induce a temporary shift in the perception of sound location. This phenomenon, known as the 'ventriloquist aftereffect', reflects a realignment of auditory and visual spatial representations such that they approach perceptual alignment despite their physical spatial discordance. Such dynamic changes to sensory representations are likely to underlie the brain's ability to accommodate inter-sensory discordance produced by sensory errors (particularly in sound localization) (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  20.  13
    The structure of the visual world. I. Space-perception and the perception of wholes.D. M. Purdy - 1935 - Psychological Review 42 (5):399-424.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21. The geometry of visual space.Robert French - 1987 - Noûs 21 (2):115-133.
  22. Merleau-Ponty’s Visual Space and the Law of Large Numbers.David Grandy - 2006 - Studia Phaenomenologica 6:391-406.
    Maurice Merleau-Ponty argued that the seeing of things together (focal figure and background objects) accounted for the sense that things possess unseen depth: they are three-dimensional entities, not facades. I compare this idea to the law of large numbers. In both cases, single entities take on substance, depth, or meaning when assimilated into a large body of comparable instances. Thinking along these lines, Erwin Schrödinger proposed that living processes achieve order by virtue of the multiplicity of their constituent parts, any (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  12
    Merleau-Ponty’s Visual Space and the Law of Large Numbers.David Grandy - 2006 - Studia Phaenomenologica 6:391-406.
    Maurice Merleau-Ponty argued that the seeing of things together (focal figure and background objects) accounted for the sense that things possess unseen depth: they are three-dimensional entities, not facades. I compare this idea to the law of large numbers. In both cases, single entities take on substance, depth, or meaning when assimilated into a large body of comparable instances. Thinking along these lines, Erwin Schrödinger proposed that living processes achieve order by virtue of the multiplicity of their constituent parts, any (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  39
    Studies in space orientation. III. Perception of the upright in the absence of a visual field.H. A. Witkin & S. E. Asch - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (5):603.
  25.  42
    Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Peception of Shape, Space and Appearance.Liliana Albertazzi (ed.) - 2013 - Chichester, West Sussex, UK: Wiley.
    Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance Liliana Albertazzi. the sort I have in mind. What I am speaking of is the mandatory correlations between attributes of visual space (those of, e.g., surfaces, shape, distance, direction) and  ...
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  26.  96
    Intuition and construction in Berkeley's account of visual space.Lorne Falkenstein - 1994 - Journal of the History of Philosophy 32 (1):63-84.
    This paper examines Berkeley's attitude toward our perception of spatial relations on the two- dimensional visual field. This is a topic on which there has been some controversy. Historians of visual theory have tended to suppose that Berkeley took "all" spatial relations to be derived in the way our knowledge of depth is: from association of more primitive sensations which are themselves in no way spatial. But many philosophers commenting on Berkeley have supposed that he takes our (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  27.  13
    Characterizing the perception of urban spaces from visual analytics of street-level imagery.Frederico Freitas, Todd Berreth, Yi-Chun Chen & Arnav Jhala - 2023 - AI and Society 38 (4):1361-1371.
    This project uses machine learning and computer vision techniques and a novel interactive visualization tool to provide street-level characterization of urban spaces such as safety and maintenance in urban neighborhoods. This is achieved by collecting and annotating street-view images, extracting objective metrics through computer vision techniques, and using crowdsourcing to statistically model the perception of subjective metrics such as safety and maintenance. For modeling human perception and scaling it up with a predictive algorithm, we evaluate perception predictions (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28.  36
    Studies in space orientation: I. Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields.S. E. Asch & H. A. Witkin - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (3):325.
  29.  44
    Studies in space orientation. IV. Further experiments on perception of the upright with displaced visual fields.H. A. Witkin & S. E. Asch - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (6):762.
  30.  47
    Studies in space orientation. II. Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields and with body tilted.S. E. Asch & H. A. Witkin - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (4):455.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  31. Does Visual Spatial Awareness Require the Visual Awareness of Space?John Schwenkler - 2012 - Mind and Language 27 (3):308-329.
    Many philosophers have held that it is not possible to experience a spatial object, property, or relation except against the background of an intact awareness of a space that is somehow ‘absolute’. This paper challenges that claim, by analyzing in detail the case of a brain-damaged subject whose visual experiences seem to have violated this condition: spatial objects and properties were present in his visual experience, but space itself was not. I go on to suggest that (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  32. The Perception Of The Visual World.James J. Gibson - 1950 - Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  33. Kant on the perception of space (and time).Gary Hatfield - 2006 - In Paul Guyer (ed.), The Cambridge Companion to Kant and Modern Philosophy. Cambridge University Press. pp. 61--93.
    Although the “Transcendental Aesthetic” is the briefest part of the first Critique, it has garnered a lion's share of discussion. This fact reflects the important implications that Kant drew from his arguments there. He used the arguments concerning space and time to display examples of synthetic a priori cognition, to secure his division between intuitions and concepts, and to support transcendental idealism. Earlier, in the years around 1770, Kant's investigations into space and time had facilitated his turn toward (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  34.  59
    Evaluation of the Visually Impaired Experience of the Sound Environment in Urban Spaces.Sen Zhang, Ke Zhang, Meng Zhang & Xiaoyang Liu - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Visually impaired people have unique perceptions of and usage requirements for various urban spaces. Therefore, understanding these perceptions can help create reasonable layouts and construct urban infrastructure. This study recruited 26 visually impaired volunteers to evaluate 24 sound environments regarding clarity, comfort, safety, vitality, and depression. This data was collected in seven different types of urban spaces. An independent sample non-parametric test was used to determine the significance of the differences between environmental evaluation results for each evaluation dimension and to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  17
    Mapping the Color Space of Saccadic Selectivity in Visual Search.Yun Xu, Emily C. Higgins, Mei Xiao & Marc Pomplun - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (5):877-887.
    Color coding is used to guide attention in computer displays for such critical tasks as baggage screening or air traffic control. It has been shown that a display object attracts more attention if its color is more similar to the color for which one is searching. However, what does similar precisely mean? Can we predict the amount of attention that a display color will receive during a search for a given target color? To tackle this question, two color‐search experiments measuring (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36. On seeing a material thing in space: The role of kinaesthesis in visual perception.John J. Drummond - 1979 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 40 (1):19-32.
  37.  33
    Space-time relations: Effects of time on perceived visual extent.J. Christopher Bill & Leon W. Teft - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (1):196.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  38.  25
    Space and Scale in Medieval Painting Reflects Imagination and Perception.Robert Pepperell, Alistair Burleigh & Nicole Ruta - 2022 - Gestalt Theory 44 (1-2):61-78.
    Prior to the discovery of linear perspective in the fifteenth century, European artists based their compositions more on imagination than the direct observation of nature. Medieval paintings, therefore, can be thought of as ‘mental projections’ of space rather than optical projections, and were sometimes regarded as ‘primitive’ by historians as they lacked the spatial consistency of later works based on the rules of linear perspective. There are noticeable differences in the way objects are depicted in paintings of the different (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39. The Public Character of Visual Objects: Shape Perception, Joint Attention, and Standpoint Transcendence.Axel Seemann - forthcoming - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences:1-19.
    Ordinary human perceivers know that visual objects are perceivable from standpoints other than their own. The aim of this paper is to provide an explanation of how perceptual experience equips perceivers with this knowledge. I approach the task by discussing a variety of action-based theories of perception. Some of these theories maintain that standpoint transcendence is required for shape perception. I argue that this standpoint transcendence must take place in the phenomenal present and that it can be (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  40.  71
    Empirical Conditions for a Reidean Geometry of Visual Experience.Hannes Ole Matthiessen - 2016 - Topoi 35 (2):511-522.
    Thomas Reid's Geometry of Visibles, according to which the geometrical properties of an object's perspectival appearance equal the geometrical properties of its projection on the inside of a sphere with the eye in its centre allows for two different interpretations. It may (1) be understood as a theory about phenomenal visual space – i.e. an account of how things appear to human observers from a certain point of view – or it may (2) be seen as a mathematical (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  41. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology; Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance.Liliana Albertazzi (ed.) - 2013 - Wiley-Blackwell.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  42. Perception and cognition: essays in the philosophy of psychology.Gary Carl Hatfield - 2009 - New York: Oxford University Press.
    Representation and content in some (actual) theories of perception -- Representation in perception and cognition : task analysis, psychological functions, and rule instantiation -- Perception as unconscious inference -- Representation and constraints : the inverse problem and the structure of visual space -- On perceptual constancy -- Getting objects for free (or not) : the philosophy and psychology of object perception -- Color perception and neural encoding : does metameric matching entail a loss (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  43.  10
    Aesthetic and Space Concept of Visual Composition in Interior and Architecture of Bali Madya Dwelling.A. Gede Rai Remawa, Imam Santosa & Biranul Anas Zaman - 2013 - Cultura 10 (2):157-168.
    Global era is an era of acculturation which may surface difficulties due to the tendency of becoming global chaos that may influence people’s thought. Everyonehas their own views and has made changes with their own worldview perception; hybrid and heterodox. Changes without wisdom will eliminate local elements.This phenomenon has influenced myriad forms of visual composition and architecture of Bali Madya dwelling. Balinese culture has gone through various changes since Rsi Markandeya in the 9th century, Empu Kuturan in the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  40
    Aesthetic and Space Concept of Visual Composition in Interior and Architecture of Bali Madya Dwelling.A. A. Gede Rai Remawa, Imam Santosa & Biranul Anas Zaman - 2013 - Cultura 10 (2):157-168.
    Global era is an era of acculturation which may surface difficulties due to the tendency of becoming global chaos that may influence people’s thought. Everyonehas their own views and has made changes with their own worldview perception; hybrid and heterodox. Changes without wisdom will eliminate local elements.This phenomenon has influenced myriad forms of visual composition and architecture of Bali Madya dwelling. Balinese culture has gone through various changes since Rsi Markandeya in the 9th century, Empu Kuturan in the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. Consensus and controversy: Helmholtz on the visual perception of space.R. Steven Turner - 1993 - In David Cahan (ed.), Hermann von Helmholtz and the Foundations of Nineteenth-Century Science. University of California Press. pp. 154--203.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  46. The 4D Space-Time Dimensions of Facial Perception.Adelaide L. Burt & David P. Crewther - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Facial information is a powerful channel for human-to-human communication. Characteristically, faces can be defined as biological objects that are four-dimensional (4D) patterns, whereby they have concurrently a spatial structure as well as temporal dynamics. The spatial characteristics of facial objects possess three dimensions (3D), namely breadth, height and importantly, depth. The temporal properties of facial objects are defined by how a 3D facial structure evolves dynamically over time; where time is referred to as the fourth dimension (4D). Our entire perspective (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  8
    Frameworks, Artworks, Place: The Space of Perception in the Modern World.Tim Mehigan - 2008 - Rodopi.
    How space – mental, emotional, visual – is implicated in our constructions of reality and our art is the focus of this set of innovative essays. For the first time art theorists and historians, visual artists, literary critics and philosophers have come together to assay the problem of space both within conventional discipline boundaries and across them. What emerges is a stimulating discussion of the problem of embodied space and situated consciousness that will be of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  14
    Effects of training in space orientation on perception of the upright.Melvin Weiner - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (5):367.
  49.  86
    Visual Experience: Sensation, Cognition, and Constancy.Gary Hatfield & Sarah Allred (eds.) - 2012 - Oxford University Press.
    Seeing happens effortlessly and yet is endlessly complex. Among the most fascinating aspects of visual perception is its stability and constancy. As we shift our gaze or move about the world, the light projected onto the retinas is constantly changing. Yet the surrounding objects appear stable in their properties. Psychologists have long been interested in the constancies. They have asked questions such as: How good is constancy? Is constancy a fact about how things look, or is it a (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  50. Spatial Perception and Geometry in Kant and Helmholtz.Gary Hatfield - 1984 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1984:569 - 587.
    This paper examines Helmholtz's attempt to use empirical psychology to refute certain of Kant's epistemological positions. Particularly, Helmholtz believed that his work in the psychology of visual perception showed Kant's doctrine of the a priori character of spatial intuition to be in error. Some of Helmholtz's arguments are effective, but this effectiveness derives from his arguments to show the possibility of obtaining evidence that the structure of physical space is non-Euclidean, and these arguments do not depend on (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 1000