Results for 'Spatial task'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  31
    Spatial task context makes short-latency reaches prone to induced Roelofs illusion.Bahareh Taghizadeh & Alexander Gail - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  2.  24
    Corrigendum: Spatial task context makes short-latency reaches prone to induced Roelofs illusion.Bahareh Taghizadeh & Alexander Gail - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  3.  14
    Testosterone, dihydrotestosterone, and spatial task performances of males.Walter F. McKeever & Richard A. Deyo - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (4):305-308.
  4.  15
    Map for accomplishing spatial tasks.Osamu Hoshino, Takafumi Yoshizawa & Takeshi Kambara - 2002 - In Kunio Yasue, Marj Jibu & Tarcisio Della Senta (eds.), No Matter, Never Mind. John Benjamins. pp. 33--289.
  5. The Effect of Subject's Sophistication on Responses to Spatial Tasks', Le dessin technique.J. B. DeregowskiI & S. Dziurawiec - forthcoming - Hermes.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  40
    A spatially oriented decision does not induce consciousness in a motor task.Bruce Bridgeman & Valerie Huemer - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):454-464.
    Visual information follows at least two branches in the human nervous system, following a common input stage: a cognitive ''what'' branch governs perception and experience, while a sensorimotor ''how'' branch handles visually guided behavior though its outputs are unconscious. The sensorimotor system is probed with an isomorphic task, requiring a 1:1 relationship between target position and motor response. The cognitive system, in contrast, is probed with a forced qualitative decision, expressed verbally, about the location of a target. Normally, the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  7.  34
    Improving spatial abilities through mindfulness: Effects on the mental rotation task.Liuna Geng, Lei Zhang & Diheng Zhang - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (3):801-806.
    In this study, we demonstrate a previously unknown finding that mindful learning can improve an individual’s spatial cognition without regard to gender differences. Thirty-two volunteers participated in the experiment. Baselines for spatial ability were first measured for the reaction time on the mental rotation task. Next, the participants were randomly assigned to either a mindful or mindless learning condition. After learning, the mental rotation task showed that those in the mindful learning condition responded faster than those (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  8.  13
    The Spatial Learning Task of Lhermitte and Signoret (1972): Normative Data in Adults Aged 18–45.Alana Collins, Michael M. Saling, Sarah J. Wilson, Graeme D. Jackson & Chris Tailby - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13:860982.
    ObjectiveThe Spatial Learning Task of Lhermitte and Signoret is an object-location arbitrary associative learning task. The task was originally developed to evaluate adults with severe amnesia. It is currently used in populations where the memory system either is not yet fully developed or where it has been compromised (e.g. epilepsy, traumatic brain injury, electroconvulsive therapy, cerebrovascular disease and dementia). Normative data have been published for paediatric cohorts and for older adults, however no data exist for the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  78
    Visuo-spatial and verbal working memory in the five-disc tower of London task: An individual differences approach.K. J. Gilhooly, V. Wynn, L. H. Phillips, R. H. Logie & S. Della Sala - 2002 - Thinking and Reasoning 8 (3):165 – 178.
    This paper reports a study of the roles of visuo-spatial and verbal working memory capacities in solving a planning task - the five-disc Tower of London (TOL) task. An individual differences approach was taken. Sixty adult participants were tested on 20 TOL tasks of varying difficulty. Total moves over the 20 TOL tasks was taken as a measure of performance. Participants were also assessed on measures of fluid intelligence (Raven's matrices), verbal short-term storage (Digit span), verbal working (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  10.  97
    Spatial awareness, alertness, and ADHD: The re-emergence of unilateral neglect with time-on-task.Melanie A. George, Veronika B. Dobler, Elaine Nicholls & Tom Manly - 2005 - Brain and Cognition 57 (3):264-275.
  11.  74
    Studying Spatial Visual Attention: The Attention-Window Task as a Measurement Tool for the Shape and Maximum Spread of the Attention Window.Stefanie Klatt & Daniel Memmert - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Visual attentional processes have been an important topic in psychological research for years. Over the last few decades, new methods have been developed, aiming to explore the characteristics of the focus of attention in more detail. Studies that applied the “Attention-Window Task” quantified the maximum extent of the “Attention Window” along its horizontal, vertical, and diagonal meridians, when subjects were required to perceive two peripheral stimuli simultaneously. In three experiments using the AWT, we investigated the effects of cue validity, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  20
    Spatial and Spectral Auditory Temporal-Order Judgment Tasks in Elderly People Are Performed Using Different Perceptual Strategies.Elzbieta Szelag, Katarzyna Jablonska, Magdalena Piotrowska, Aneta Szymaszek & Hanna Bednarek - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  13.  18
    Task dependent spatial memory across saccades.Keith S. Karn, Joel Lachter, Per Møller & Mary Hayhoe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):267-268.
  14. Spatial, temporal, and modulatory factors affecting GasNet evolvability in a visually guided robotics task.Philip Husbands, Andrew Philippides, Patricia Vargas, Christopher L. Buckley, Peter Fine, Ezequiel Di Paolo & Michael O'Shea - 2010 - Complexity 16 (2):35-44.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15.  43
    Global interference and spatial uncertainty in the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART).William S. Helton, Lena Weil, Annette Middlemiss & Andrew Sawers - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (1):77-85.
    The Sustained Attention to Response Task is a Go–No-Go signal detection task developed to measure lapses of sustained conscious attention. In this study, we examined the impact global interference and spatial uncertainty has on SART performance. Ten participants performed either a SART or a traditionally formatted version of a global–local stimuli detection task with spatially certain and uncertain signals. Reaction time in the SART was insensitive to global interference and spatial uncertainty, whereas reaction time in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  16.  18
    ERP evidence for task modulations on face perceptual processing at different spatial scales.Valérie Goffaux, Boutheina Jemel, Corentin Jacques, Bruno Rossion & Philippe G. Schyns - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (2):313-325.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  17.  33
    Verbal counting and spatial strategies in numerical tasks: Evidence from indigenous australia.Brian Butterworth & Robert Reeve - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (4):443 – 457.
    In this study, we test whether children whose culture lacks CWs and counting practices use a spatial strategy to support enumeration tasks. Children from two indigenous communities in Australia whose native and only language (Warlpiri or Anindilyakwa) lacked CWs and were tested on classical number development tasks, and the results were compared with those of children reared in an English-speaking environment. We found that Warlpiri- and Anindilyakwa-speaking children performed equivalently to their English-speaking counterparts. However, in tasks in which they (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  18.  21
    Identification and location tasks rely on different mental processes: a diffusion model account of validity effects in spatial cueing paradigms with emotional stimuli.Roland Imhoff, Jens Lange & Markus Germar - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (2):231-244.
    ABSTRACTSpatial cueing paradigms are popular tools to assess human attention to emotional stimuli, but different variants of these paradigms differ in what participants’ primary task is. In one variant, participants indicate the location of the target, whereas in the other they indicate the shape of the target. In the present paper we test the idea that although these two variants produce seemingly comparable cue validity effects on response times, they rest on different underlying processes. Across four studies using both (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  19.  23
    Flowers and spiders in spatial stimulus-response compatibility: does affective valence influence selection of task-sets or selection of responses?Motonori Yamaguchi, Jing Chen, Scott Mishler & Robert W. Proctor - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (5):1003-1017.
    ABSTRACTThe present study examined the effect of stimulus valence on two levels of selection in the cognitive system, selection of a task-set and selection of a response. In the first experiment, participants performed a spatial compatibility task in which stimulus-response mappings were determined by stimulus valence. There was a standard spatial stimulus-response compatibility effect for positive stimuli and a reversed SRC effect for negative stimuli, but the same data could be interpreted as showing faster responses when (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20. Construction and Revision of Spatial Mental Models under High Task Demand.Jelica Nejasmic, Leandra Bucher, Paul D. Thorn & Markus Knauff - 2014 - In Paul Bello, Marcello Guarini, Marjorie McShane & Brian Scassellati (eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 1066-72.
    Individuals often revise their beliefs when confronted with contradicting evidence. Belief revision in the spatial domain can be regarded as variation of initially constructed spatial mental models. Construction and revision usually follow distinct cognitive principles. The present study examines whether principles of revisions which follow constructions under high task demands differ from principles applied after less demanding constructions. We manipulated the task demands for model constructions by means of the continuity with which a spatial model (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  14
    The Development of Spatial Memory Analyzed by Means of Ecological Walking Task.Pierpaolo Sorrentino, Anna Lardone, Matteo Pesoli, Marianna Liparoti, Simone Montuori, Giuseppe Curcio, Giuseppe Sorrentino, Laura Mandolesi & Francesca Foti - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  22.  22
    Oculomotor involvement in spatial working memory is task-specific.Keira Ball, David G. Pearson & Daniel T. Smith - 2013 - Cognition 129 (2):439-446.
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  23.  11
    Relational but not spatial memory: The task at hand.Elisabeth A. Murray - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):489-490.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  49
    Strategies for Human‐Driven Robot Comprehension of Spatial Descriptions by Older Adults in a Robot Fetch Task.Laura Carlson, Marjorie Skubic, Jared Miller, Zhiyu Huo & Tatiana Alexenko - 2014 - Topics in Cognitive Science 6 (3):513-533.
    This contribution presents a corpus of spatial descriptions and describes the development of a human-driven spatial language robot system for their comprehension. The domain of application is an eldercare setting in which an assistive robot is asked to “fetch” an object for an elderly resident based on a natural language spatial description given by the resident. In Part One, we describe a corpus of naturally occurring descriptions elicited from a group of older adults within a virtual 3D (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  25.  14
    Control of spatial orienting: Context-specific proportion cued effects in an exogenous spatial cueing task.Alex Gough, Jesse Garcia, Maryem Torres-Quesada & Bruce Milliken - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 30:220-233.
  26.  31
    Learning and performance on a key-pressing task as function of the degree of spatial stimulus-response correspondence.Robert E. Morin & David A. Grant - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (1):39.
  27.  9
    What Individuals Experience During Visuo-Spatial Working Memory Task Performance: An Exploratory Phenomenological Study.Aleš Oblak, Anka Slana Ozimič, Grega Repovš & Urban Kordeš - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    In experimental cognitive psychology, objects of inquiry are typically operationalized with psychological tasks. When interpreting results from such tasks, we focus primarily on behavioral measures such as reaction times and accuracy rather than experiences – i.e., phenomenology – associated with the task, and posit that the tasks elicit the desired cognitive phenomenon. Evaluating whether the tasks indeed elicit the desired phenomenon can be facilitated by understanding the experience during task performance. In this paper we explore the breadth of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  28.  23
    Disentangling Spatial Metaphors for Time Using Non-spatial Responses and Auditory Stimuli.Esther J. Walker, Benjamin K. Bergen & Rafael Núñez - 2014 - Metaphor and Symbol 29 (4):316-327.
    While we often talk about time using spatial terms, experimental investigation of space-time associations has focused primarily on the space in front of the participant. This has had two consequences: the disregard of the space behind the participant and the creation of potential task demands produced by spatialized manual button-presses. We introduce and test a new paradigm that uses auditory stimuli and vocal responses to address these issues. Participants made temporal judgments about deictic or sequential relationships presented auditorily (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  29.  44
    Is the sunny side up and the dark side down? Effects of stimulus type and valence on a spatial detection task.Maria Amorim & Ana P. Pinheiro - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (2):346-360.
    ABSTRACTIn verbal communication, affective information is commonly conveyed to others through spatial terms. This study used a target location discrimination task with neutral, positive and negative stimuli to test the automaticity of the emotion-space association, both in the vertical and horizontal spatial axes. The effects of stimulus type on emotion-space representations were also probed. A congruency effect was observed in the vertical axis: detection of upper targets preceded by positive stimuli was faster. This effect occurred for all (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30.  8
    Position mediated transfer between serial learning and a spatial discrimination task.Sheldon M. Ebenholtz - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (6):603.
  31.  21
    Mechanism of the SNARC Effect in Numerical Magnitude, Time Sequence, and Spatial Sequence Tasks: Involvement of LTM and WM.Qiangqiang Wang, Mowei Liu, Wendian Shi & Jingmei Kang - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  13
    Visual pattern matching: An investigation of some effects of decision task, auditory codability, and spatial correspondence.R. S. Nickerson & R. W. Pew - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (1):36.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  17
    Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks.L. Hermer-Vazquez - 2001 - Cognition 79 (3):263-299.
  34.  69
    Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach–withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance.Jeremy R. Gray - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (3):436.
  35.  33
    Corrigendum: The Binocular Balance at High Spatial Frequencies as Revealed by the Binocular Orientation Combination Task.Yonghua Wang, Zhifen He, Yunjie Liang, Yiya Chen, Ling Gong, Yu Mao, Xiaoxin Chen, Zhimo Yao, Daniel P. Spiegel, Jia Qu, Fan Lu, Jiawei Zhou & Robert F. Hess - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  36.  37
    The Binocular Balance at High Spatial Frequencies as Revealed by the Binocular Orientation Combination Task.Yonghua Wang, Zhifen He, Yunjie Liang, Yiya Chen, Ling Gong, Yu Mao, Xiaoxin Chen, Zhimo Yao, Daniel P. Spiegel, Jia Qu, Fan Lu, Jiawei Zhou & Robert F. Hess - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  37.  68
    Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task.Jean-Philippe van Dijck, Wim Gevers & Wim Fias - 2009 - Cognition 113 (2):248-253.
  38.  9
    Resting-State Functional Connectivity in the Dorsal Attention Network Relates to Behavioral Performance in Spatial Attention Tasks and May Show Task-Related Adaptation.Björn Machner, Lara Braun, Jonathan Imholz, Philipp J. Koch, Thomas F. Münte, Christoph Helmchen & Andreas Sprenger - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Between-subject variability in cognitive performance has been related to inter-individual differences in functional brain networks. Targeting the dorsal attention network we questioned whether resting-state functional connectivity within the DAN can predict individual performance in spatial attention tasks and whether there is short-term adaptation of DAN-FC in response to task engagement. Twenty-seven participants first underwent resting-state fMRI, they subsequently performed different tasks of spatial attention [including visual search ] and immediately afterwards received another rs-fMRI. Intra- and inter-hemispheric FC (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  26
    Effect of practice on a Stroop-like spatial directions task.Ronald E. Shor, Richard P. Hatch, Laurel J. Hudson, David T. Landrigan & Howard J. Shaffer - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 94 (2):168.
  40.  8
    Factors Influencing Saccadic Reaction Time: Effect of Task Modality, Stimulus Saliency, Spatial Congruency of Stimuli, and Pupil Size.Shimpei Yamagishi & Shigeto Furukawa - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
    It is often assumed that the reaction time of a saccade toward visual and/or auditory stimuli reflects the sensitivities of our oculomotor-orienting system to stimulus saliency. Endogenous factors, as well as stimulus-related factors, would also affect the saccadic reaction time. However, it was not clear how these factors interact and to what extent visual and auditory-targeting saccades are accounted for by common mechanisms. The present study examined the effect of, and the interaction between, stimulus saliency and audiovisual spatial congruency (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41. Reconsidering 'spatial memory' and the Morris water maze.Jacqueline Anne Sullivan - 2010 - Synthese 177 (2):261-283.
    The Morris water maze has been put forward in the philosophy of neuroscience as an example of an experimental arrangement that may be used to delineate the cognitive faculty of spatial memory (e.g., Craver and Darden, Theory and method in the neurosciences, University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, 2001; Craver, Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2007). However, in the experimental and review literature on the water maze throughout the history of its (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  42.  40
    A unity of the self or a multiplicity of locations? How the graphesthesia task sheds light on the role of spatial perspectives in bodily self-consciousness.Gabriel Arnold, Charles Spence & Malika Auvray - 2017 - Consciousness and Cognition 56:100-114.
  43.  14
    Psychophysical evidence for distinct contributions in processing low and high spatial frequencies of fearful facial expressions in backward masking task.Agata Sobków & Remigiusz Szczepanowski - 2012 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 43 (3):167-172.
    The present report examined the hypothesis that two distinct visual routes contribute in processing low and high spatial frequencies of fearful facial expressions. Having the participants presented with a backwardly masked task, we analyzed conscious processing of spatial frequency contents of emotional faces according to both objective and subjective taskrelevant criteria. It was shown that fear perception in the presence of the low-frequency faces can be supported by stronger automaticity leading to less false positives. In contrary, the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  25
    Disentangling attention from action in the emotional spatial cueing task.Manon Mulckhuyse & Geert Crombez - 2014 - Cognition and Emotion 28 (7):1223-1241.
  45.  7
    Path Learning in Individuals With Down Syndrome: The Floor Matrix Task and the Role of Individual Visuo-Spatial Measures.Chiara Meneghetti, Enrico Toffalini, Silvia Lanfranchi & Barbara Carretti - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  46.  26
    Your brain on speed: cognitive performance of a spatial working memory task is not affected by walking speed.Julia E. Kline, Katherine Poggensee & Daniel P. Ferris - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  47.  21
    The conjunction of non-consciously perceived object identity and spatial position can be retained during a visual short-term memory task.Fredrik Bergström & Johan Eriksson - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  48.  14
    Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Baseline and Slope of Prefrontal Cortex Hemodynamics During a Spatial Working Memory Task.Ryan McKendrick, Brian Falcone, Melissa Scheldrup & Hasan Ayaz - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  49. Do Gender-Related Stereotypes Affect Spatial Performance? Exploring When, How and to Whom Using a Chronometric Two-Choice Mental Rotation Task.Carla Sanchis-Segura, Naiara Aguirre, Álvaro J. Cruz-Gómez, Noemí Solozano & Cristina Forn - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  18
    Spatial Thinking in Term and Preterm-Born Preschoolers: Relations to Parent–Child Speech and Gesture.Sam Clingan-Siverly, Paige M. Nelson, Tilbe Göksun & Ö. Ece Demir-Lira - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Spatial skills predict important life outcomes, such as mathematical achievement or entrance into Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics disciplines. Children significantly vary in their spatial performance even before they enter formal schooling. One correlate of children's spatial performance is the spatial language they produce and hear from others, such as their parents. Because the emphasis has been on spatial language, less is known about the role of hand gestures in children's spatial development. Some children (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 1000