Results for 'Direct action'

989 found
Order:
  1. Goal‐Directed Action.Alfred R. Mele - 2003 - In Motivation and agency. New York: Oxford University Press.
    Argues for a constraint on a proper theory of motivation – namely, that proper motivational explanations of goal‐directed actions are causal explanations. The chapter criticizes the thesis that acceptable teleological explanations of actions are not causal explanations and it offers a solution to a problem that deviant causal chains pose for a causal theory of action.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2. Goal-Directed Action: Teleological Explanations, Causal Theories, and Deviance.Alfred R. Mele - 2000 - Noûs 34 (s14):279 - 300.
    Teleological explanations of human actions are explanations in terms of aims, goals, or purposes of human agents. According to a familiar causal approach to analyzing and explaining human action, our actions are, essentially, events (and sometimes states, perhaps) that are suitably caused by appropriate mental items, or neural realizations of those items. Causalists traditionally appeal, in part, to such goal-representing states as desires and intentions (or their neural realizers) in their explanations of human actions, and they take accept-able teleological (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  3.  32
    Directed action and animal communication.Daisie Radner - 1993 - Ration 6 (2):135-54.
    Human action theory, with its emphasis on intentions and reasons, does little to enhance our understanding of the actions of nonhuman animals. Many animal (and human) actions are directed to objects in the world, including other animals. The notion of directedness can be analysed without attributing intentions or reasons to the agent. An action is directed to object X if and only if: (1) the agent singles out X, either by orientation or by selective performance of the (...) in the presence of X; (2) the agent recognizes X as a suitable object; and (3) the goal of the action is that X should be in a certain relation to the agent or to some other object. The goal of an action is not necessarily attributable to the agent as the agent's goal in acting. Moreover, an agent can have a goal in acting without understanding how the action achieves the goal. The usefulness of the concept of directed action in the study of animal communication is illustrated with examples from the recent ethological literature. (shrink)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  4.  25
    Direct Action (1912?).Voltairine de Cleyre - unknown
    for human progress to pursue, if it is to be progress at all, who, having such a route on his mind’s map, has endeavored to point it out to others; to make them see it as he sees it; who in so doing has chosen what appeared to him clear and simple expressions to convey his thoughts to others. – to such a one it appears matter for regret and confusion of spirit that the phrase “Direct Action” has suddenly (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  5.  16
    Direct Action and Democratic Politics.Robert Benewick & Trevor Smith - 1972 - Routledge.
    First published in 1972. Militant protest is not new to British politics, but the widespread recourse to direct action, in Britain and abroad, is unprecedented. This book was the first comprehensive examination of contemporary protest in the British context. The contributors represented leading agencies of protest as well as those academics who had made this phenomenon their special concern. The result is a unique blend of direct experience and objective reflection. The first part of the volume covers (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  20
    Directed Action and Animal Communication.Daisie Radner - 1993 - Ratio 6 (2):135-154.
    Human action theory, with its emphasis on intentions and reasons, does little to enhance our understanding of the actions of nonhuman animals. Many animal (and human) actions are directed to objects in the world, including other animals. The notion of directedness can be analysed without attributing intentions or reasons to the agent. An action is directed to object X if and only if: (1) the agent singles out X, either by orientation or by selective performance of the (...) in the presence of X; (2) the agent recognizes X as a suitable object; and (3) the goal of the action is that X should be in a certain relation to the agent or to some other object. The goal of an action is not necessarily attributable to the agent as the agent's goal in acting. Moreover, an agent can have a goal in acting without understanding how the action achieves the goal. The usefulness of the concept of directed action in the study of animal communication is illustrated with examples from the recent ethological literature. (shrink)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  7.  56
    Direct Action and the Climate Crisis.Reed M. Kurtz - 2020 - Radical Philosophy Review 23 (2):261-297.
    How should we conceptualize direct action against climate change? Although direct action is an increasingly significant tactic by the global climate movement, we lack understanding how direct action contributes to the systemic change necessary for addressing the crisis. Drawing upon critical theories of climate change as a crisis in the social reproduction of the metabolic relations between humans and nature in capitalism, I conceptualize direct action as attempts to intervene directly in the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  5
    Goal-directed action in hemispatial neglect.Marlene Behrmann & Daniel V. Meegan - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):381-409.
  9. Goal-directed Action and Eligible Forms of Embodiment.D. Vernon - 2013 - Constructivist Foundations 9 (1):85-85.
    Open peer commentary on the article “Investigating Extended Embodiment Using a Computational Model and Human Experimentation” by Yuki Sato, Hiroyuki Iizuka & Takashi Ikegami. Upshot: The target article’s findings on the focus of attention in extending an agent’s body schema are consistent with those in developmental psychology and neuroscience on goal-directed action. The consequences of these findings are that embodiment can be extended in a variety of ways, not all of which require direct physical manipulation.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. Directive action and life.Ralph S. Lillie - 1937 - Philosophy of Science 4 (2):202-226.
    When we consider closely any highly integrated vital process, like embryonic development, or animal behavior of the end-subserving or purposive type, we are inevitably impressed with the importance of those special controlling factors, collectively termed “regulative,” which appear chiefly responsible for the unified and finalistic character of the whole sequence of events. These factors are persistent in their influence although they may act intermittently. Without their presence the sequence would soon lose coördination and “run wild,” just as an automobile runs (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  11.  5
    F Direct Action.Dave Foreman - forthcoming - Environmental Ethics: The Big Questions.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  32
    Direct Action and Democracy Today.Jeffrey D. Hilmer - 2010 - Theoria: A Journal of Social and Political Theory 57 (124):113-115.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  13
    The direct action of rewards upon mental connections and their indirect action via the stimulation of inner equivalents of the connections.E. L. Thorndike - 1935 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 18 (1):91.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14. The organization of goal-directed action: A research report.M. Von Cranach, Elfie Machler & Vera Steiner - 1985 - In G. P. Ginsburg, Marylin Brenner & Mario von Cranach (eds.), Discovery Strategies in the Psychology of Action. Academic Press. pp. 19--61.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  21
    Infants' understanding of object-directed action.Ann T. Phillips & Henry M. Wellman - 2005 - Cognition 98 (2):137-155.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  16.  42
    Newborns’ preference for goal-directed actions.Laila Craighero, Irene Leo, Carlo Umiltà & Francesca Simion - 2011 - Cognition 120 (1):26-32.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  17.  16
    Infants’ Understanding of Object-Directed Action: An Interdisciplinary Synthesis.Scott J. Robson & Valerie A. Kuhlmeier - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  10
    Ecology, Dharma and Direct Action: A Brief Survey of Contemporary Eco-Buddhist Activism in Korea.Young-Hae Yoon & Sherwin Jones - 2015 - Buddhist Studies Review 31 (2):293-311.
    Over the last few decades there has emerged a small, yet influential eco-Buddhism movement in South Korea which, since the turn of the millennium, has seen several S?n Buddhist clerics engage in high-profile protests and activism campaigns opposing massive development projects which threatened widespread ecological destruction. This article will survey the issues and events surrounding three such protests; the 2003 samboilbae, or ‘threesteps- one-bow’, march led by Venerable Suky?ng against the Saemangeum Reclamation Project, Venerable Jiyul’s Anti-Mt. Ch?ns?ng tunnel hunger-strike campaign (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  42
    The ties that bind? Self‐ and place‐identity in environmental direct action.Jon Anderson - 2004 - Ethics, Place and Environment 7 (1-2):45 – 57.
    This paper explores what happens to the identity of self when entering a place of protest, and what happens to it on leaving. In short, it explores the relations between identities of self and place. Acknowledging the presence of a multiplicity of identities in relation to both notions, it examines the ways in which aspects of the self influence place, and conversely, how aspects of place influence the self. By using empirical examples from Environmental Direct Action, the paper (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  20.  63
    8 Goal-Directed Action and Teleological Explanation.Scott R. Sehon - 2007 - In J. K. Campbell, M. O'Rourke & H. S. Silverstein (eds.), Causation and Explanation. MIT Press. pp. 4--155.
  21. Activism and Direct Action Politics.Nik Heynen & Levi Van Sant - 2015 - In Thomas Albert Perreault, Gavin Bridge & James McCarthy (eds.), The Routledge handbook of political ecology. New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  16
    The Crisis of the Humanities and the Viability of Direct Action.Nathan Eckstrand - 2021 - Radical Philosophy Review 24 (2):135-167.
    Humanities advocates focus on demonstrating the humanities’ value to encourage participation. This advocacy is largely done through institutional means, and rarely taken directly to the public. This article argues that by reframing the theory of Direct Action, humanities advocates can effectively engage the public. The article begins by exploring three different understandings of the humanities: that they develop good citizenship, that they develop understanding, and that they develop critical thought. The article then discusses what Direct Action (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  22
    Religiosity and Public Reason: The Case of Direct Action Animal Rights Advocacy.J. Hadley - 2017 - Res Publica 23 (3):299-312.
    Recent social science research indicates that animal rights philosophy plays the functional role of a religion in the lives of the most committed animal rights advocates. In this paper, I apply the functional religion thesis to the recent debate over the place of direct action animal rights advocacy in democratic theory. I outline the usefulness of the functional religion thesis and explain its implications for theorists that call for deliberative theories to be more inclusive of coercive forms of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  24. Making no appeal to the state: ending animal abuse through total liberation and direct action.Will Boisseau - 2021 - In Anthony J. Nocella & Amber E. George (eds.), Critical Animal Studies and Social Justice: Critical Theory, Dismantling Speciesism, and Total Liberation. Lanham: Lexington Books.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  7
    Books in review : Direct action and liberal democracy by April Carter. New York: Harper and row, 1973. Pp. 170. $9.00 hardcover; $3.25 paper. [REVIEW]Alan Ritter - 1974 - Political Theory 2 (3):354-355.
  26.  28
    The Weight of Emotions in Decision-Making: How Fearful and Happy Facial Stimuli Modulate Action Readiness of Goal-Directed Actions.Giovanni Mirabella - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  27.  9
    The Effect of Cognitive Relevance of Directed Actions on Mathematical Reasoning.Candace Walkington, Mitchell J. Nathan, Min Wang & Kelsey Schenck - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (9):e13180.
    Theories of grounded and embodied cognition offer a range of accounts of how reasoning and body‐based processes are related to each other. To advance theories of grounded and embodied cognition, we explore the cognitive relevance of particular body states to associated math concepts. We test competing models of action‐cognition transduction to investigate the cognitive relevance of directed actions to students’ mathematical reasoning in the area of geometry. The hypotheses we test include (1) that cognitively relevant directed actions have a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  21
    The Climate Emergency: Are the Doctors who take Non-violent Direct Action to Raise Public Awareness Radical Activists, Rightminded Professionals, or Reluctant Whistleblowers?Terry Kemple - 2020 - The New Bioethics 26 (2):111-124.
    When doctors become aware of a threat to public health, they have a professional duty to try to mitigate the threat. Climate change is a recognized major threat to planetary and public health that...
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  29.  17
    Editorial: Infants' Understanding and Production of Goal-Directed Actions in the Context of Social and Object-Related Interactions.Daniela Corbetta & Jacqueline Fagard - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  9
    Age-Related Changes in the Neural Processes of Reward-Directed Action and Inhibition of Action.Thang M. Le, Herta Chao, Ifat Levy & Chiang-Shan R. Li - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31.  11
    Explicit Sense of Agency in an Automatic Control Situation: Effects of Goal-Directed Action and the Gradual Emergence of Outcome.Ryoichi Nakashima & Takatsune Kumada - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  41
    Coordinate transformations in the genesis of directed action.C. R. Gallistel - 1999 - In Benjamin Martin Bly & David E. Rumelhart (eds.), Cognitive Science. Academic Press. pp. 1-43.
  33.  58
    From discrete actors to goal-directed actions: Toward a process-based methodology for psychology.Endre E. Kadar & Judith A. Effken - 2005 - Philosophical Psychology 18 (3):353 – 382.
    Studying social phenomena is often assumed to be inherently different from studying natural science phenomena. In psychology, this assumption has led to a division of the field into social and experimental domains. The same kind of division has carried over into ecological psychology, despite the fact that Gibson clearly intended his theory for both social and natural phenomena. In this paper, we argue that the social/natural science dichotomy can be derived from a distinction between hermeneutics and science that is deeply (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  34.  21
    The planar mosaic fails to account for spatially directed action.Roberta L. Klatzky & Nicholas A. Giudice - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):554 - 555.
    Humans' spatial representations enable navigation and reaching to targets above the ground plane, even without direct perceptual support. Such abilities are inconsistent with an impoverished representation of the third dimension. Features that differentiate humans from most terrestrial animals, including raised eye height and arms dedicated to manipulation rather than locomotion, have led to robust metric representations of volumetric space.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  24
    The Phenomenology of Eye Movement Intentions and their Disruption in Goal-Directed Actions.Maximilian Roszko, Lars Hall, Petter Johansson & Philip Pärnamets - 2018 - In Timothy M. Rogers, Marina Rau, Jerry Zhu & Chuck Kalish (eds.), Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 973-978.
    The role of intentions in motor planning is heavily weighted in classical psychological theories, but their role in generating eye movements, and our awareness of these oculomotor intentions, has not been investigated explicitly. In this study, the extent to which we monitor oculomotor intentions, i.e. the intentions to shift one’s gaze towards a specific location, and whether they can be expressed in conscious experience, is investigated. A forced-choice decision task was developed where a pair of faces moved systematically across a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  11
    Dopaminergic modulation of positive expectations for goal-directed action: evidence from Parkinson’s disease.Noham Wolpe, Cristina Nombela & James B. Rowe - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  37.  11
    Similar Mechanisms of Movement Control in Target- and Effect-Directed Actions toward Spatial Goals?Andrea M. Walter & Martina Rieger - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  11
    To Train or to Entertain the Brain: How Does Enhanced Focus of Attention Guide Perception into the Goal Directed Action.Gorjup Rado & Gorjup Niko - 2017 - Philosophy Study 7 (8).
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  18
    Input-output relations in goal-directed actions.M. Jeannerod - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):628-629.
  40.  22
    Social meaning in the observation of goal directed action.Ladislav Valach, Mario von Cranach & Urs Kalbermatten - 1988 - Semiotica 71 (3-4):243-260.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  18
    The Politics of Legal Abortion: From Direct Action to Dialogue.Jeffrey A. Gauthier - 2021 - Hypatia 36 (4):800-804.
    In her highly influential 1984 study Abortion and the Politics of Motherhood, Kristin Luker speculates that opposition to legal abortion among women was likely to be strongest among those who were full-time homemakers without a college education. But despite a marked decline in that demographic group and a well-documented rise in public support for gender equality since then, the rate of support for legal abortion has remained stubbornly fixed at between fifty and fifty-five percent. This tepid support has coincided with (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  11
    What are the consequences of understanding the complex goal-directed actions of others?Mary Gauvain - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (5):700-701.
    Four issues that build on the ideas offered by Tomasello et al. are discussed: the developmental course of shared intentionality and its relation to other developing abilities and experiences, and the conceptualization of three key features of the process: motivation, plans and the development of planning, and culture.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43. Generality and perceptual constraints in understanding goal-directed actions in young infants.I. Király, B. Jovanovic, G. Aschersleben, W. Prinz & G. Gergely - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (4):752-769.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44. The directional bias in children's line copying does not generalise to pointing actions.A. Davis & B. De Bruyn - 2004 - In Robert Schwartz (ed.), Perception. Malden Ma: Blackwell. pp. 168-168.
  45. Action-Directed Pragmatics Secures Semantically Autonomous Knowledge.Igal Kvart - manuscript
    In the past couple of decades, there were a few major attempts to establish the thesis of pragmatic infringement – that a significant pragmatic ingredient figures significantly in the truth-conditions for knowledge-ascriptions. As candidates, epistemic contextualism and Relativism flaunted conversational standards, and Stanley's SSI promoted stakes. These conceptions were propelled first and foremost by obviously pragmatic examples of knowledge ascriptions that seem to require a pragmatic component in the truth-conditions of knowledge ascriptions in order to be accounted for. However, if (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  46.  6
    Directives: Entitlement and contingency in action.Jonathan Potter & Alexandra Craven - 2010 - Discourse Studies 12 (4):419-442.
    This article is focused on the nature of directives. It draws on Curl and Drew’s analysis of entitlement and contingency in request types and applies this to a corpus of directives that occur in UK family mealtimes involving parents and young children. While requests are built as contingent to varying degrees on the recipient’s willingness or ability to comply, directives embody no orientation to the recipient’s ability or desire to perform the relevant activity. This lack of orientation to ability or (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  47. From direct perception to the primacy of action: a closer look at James Gibson's ecological approach to psychology.Alan Costall - 2004 - In Gavin Bremner & Alan Slater (eds.), Theories of Infant Development. Blackwell. pp. 70--89.
  48. Action is goal-directed.Michael Brenner - 1985 - In G. P. Ginsburg, Marylin Brenner & Mario von Cranach (eds.), Discovery Strategies in the Psychology of Action. Academic Press. pp. 35--207.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  24
    Action Recognition and Movement Direction Discrimination Tasks Are Associated with Different Adaptation Patterns.Stephan de la Rosa, Mina Ekramnia & Heinrich H. Bülthoff - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
  50. «Direct» and «indirect»: A reply to critics of our action theory.John Finnis, Germain Grisez & Joseph Boyle - 2001 - The Thomist 65 (1):1-44.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
1 — 50 / 989