Results for 'task instructions'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  22
    Task instructions and implicit theory of mind.Dana Schneider, Zoie E. Nott & Paul E. Dux - 2014 - Cognition 133 (1):43-47.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  2.  24
    Task instructions for anagrams following different task instructions and training.Irving Maltzman, Eugene Eisman, Lloyd O. Brooks & William M. Smith - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (6):418.
  3.  19
    Effects of task instructions on solution of different classes of anagrams.Irving Maltzman & Lloyd Morrisett Jr - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 (5):351.
  4.  60
    When you fail to see what you were told to look for: Inattentional blindness and task instructions.Anne M. Aimola Davies, Stephen Waterman, Rebekah C. White & Martin Davies - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (1):221-230.
    Inattentional blindness studies have shown that an unexpected object may go unnoticed if it does not share the property specified in the task instructions. Our aim was to demonstrate that observers develop an attentional set for a property not specified in the task instructions if it allows easier performance of the primary task. Three experiments were conducted using a dynamic selective-looking paradigm. Stimuli comprised four black squares and four white diamonds, so that shape and colour (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  5.  36
    When you fail to see what you were told to look for: Inattentional blindness and task instructions.Anne Aimola Davies, Stephen Waterman, Rebekah White & Martin Davies - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (1):221-230.
    Inattentional blindness studies have shown that an unexpected object may go unnoticed if it does not share the property specified in the task instructions. Our aim was to demonstrate that observers develop an attentional set for a property not specified in the task instructions if it allows easier performance of the primary task. Three experiments were conducted using a dynamic selective-looking paradigm. Stimuli comprised four black squares and four white diamonds, so that shape and colour (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  6. Under Pressure to Achieve? The Impact of Type and Style of Task Instructions on Student Cheating.Caroline Julia Pulfrey, Maarten Vansteenkiste & Aikaterina Michou - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
  7.  11
    Effects of Perturbation Velocity, Direction, Background Muscle Activation, and Task Instruction on Long-Latency Responses Measured From Forearm Muscles.Jacob Weinman, Paria Arfa-Fatollahkhani, Andrea Zonnino, Rebecca C. Nikonowicz & Fabrizio Sergi - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    The central nervous system uses feedback processes that occur at multiple time scales to control interactions with the environment. The long-latency response is the fastest process that directly involves cortical areas, with a motoneuron response measurable 50 ms following an imposed limb displacement. Several behavioral factors concerning perturbation mechanics and the active role of muscles prior or during the perturbation can modulate the long-latency response amplitude in the upper limbs, but the interactions among many of these factors had not been (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  31
    Information reduction during skill acquisition: The influence of task instruction.Hilde Haider & Peter A. Frensch - 1999 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 5 (2):129.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  9.  12
    The Role of General and Selective Task Instructions on Students’ Processing of Multiple Conflicting Documents.Raquel Cerdán & Maria del Carmen Marín - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  9
    Vigilance performance as related to task instructions, coaction, and knowledge of results.James M. Huntermark & Kenneth L. Witte - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12 (4):325-328.
  11.  8
    Tasks and instructions on the simulated bridge: Discourses of temporality in maritime training.Mona Lundin & Charlott Sellberg - 2018 - Discourse Studies 20 (2):289-305.
    In higher education programs that train students for professions with high standards of safety, such as aviation, shipping and healthcare, exercises in simulated environments provide opportunities for training in educational settings. This study explores the use of simulators in maritime education, taking an interest in how navigation training is achieved by using simulated environments. By conducting an interaction analysis of video data, the study examines how training students to coordinate with other vessels in traffic is topicalized in simulator exercises, focusing (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  13
    Instructions and information processing in a complex task.Paul Stager & Paul Muter - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 87 (2):291.
  13.  13
    Situational instructions and task order in recall for completed and interrupted tasks.David G. Hays - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 44 (6):434.
  14.  10
    Instructional sets and subjective criterion levels in a complex information-processing task.William C. Howell & David L. Kreidler - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (6):612.
  15.  14
    Instructed Task Demands and Utilization of Action Effect Anticipation.Robert Gaschler & Dieter Nattkemper - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  16.  19
    Individuation instructions decrease the Cross-Race Effect in a face matching task.Batra Prachi & Longstaff Mitchell - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  38
    The effect of instructions and information retrieval on accepting the premises in a conditional reasoning task.Isabelle Vadeboncoeur & Henry Markovits - 1999 - Thinking and Reasoning 5 (2):97 – 113.
    Some studies have reported that, under some circumstances, participants sometimes reject the truth of conditional premises and give incorrect uncertain conclusions to MP and MT, despite the standard instructions to assume the truth of the premises. Instructions that emphasise the logical nature of the task, on the other hand, increase the number of valid conclusions to these two inferences. In this paper, we examine two possible explanations for the influence of instructions on the production of valid (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  18.  17
    Effects of instructional set on pupillary responses during a short-term memory task.William R. Clark & David A. Johnson - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 85 (2):315.
  19.  6
    Influence of task complexity and instructions upon simple and discrimination reaction times.Joseph B. Sidowski, Ross Morgan & Gordon Eckstrand - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (2):163.
  20.  6
    Elaborative feedback and instruction improve cognitive reflection but do not transfer to related tasks.Dustin P. Calvillo, Jonathan Bratton, Victoria Velazquez, Thomas J. Smelter & Danielle Crum - 2023 - Thinking and Reasoning 29 (2):276-304.
    Cognitive reflection, or the ability to inhibit intuitive and incorrect responses in favour of correct responses, predicts performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. The present study examined interventions to improve cognitive reflection. In two experiments, college students (N = 491) were assigned to one of three conditions, completed two versions of a cognitive reflection test (CRT), and then completed transfer tasks. Between the two CRTs, some participants were provided with elaborative feedback, others were instructed to consider additional responses for (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  29
    Effect of accuracy-emphasized instructions on performance on an attribute-identification task.Bohdan K. Wasilewski - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 96 (1):199.
  22.  17
    Attentional prioritization reconfigures novel instructions into action-oriented task sets.Carlos González-García, Silvia Formica, Baptist Liefooghe & Marcel Brass - 2020 - Cognition 194 (C):104059.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  23.  14
    The effect of instruction on the length-difficulty relationship for a task involving sequential dependency.Robert Stanton French - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (2):89.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  17
    Benefits of Instructed Responding in Manual Assembly Tasks: An ERP Approach.Pavle Mijović, Vanja Ković, Maarten De Vos, Ivan Mačužić, Branislav Jeremić & Ivan Gligorijević - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
  25. Academic achievement and on‐task behavior of high school biology students instructed in a cooperative small investigative group.R. Lazarowitz, R. L. Hertz, J. H. Baird & V. Bowlden - 1988 - Science Education 72 (4):475-487.
  26.  14
    Effect of stimulus and instructional variables in an ambiguous concept-attainment task.Mary Janke - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 93 (1):21.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  13
    Effects of instructions, orienting task, and memory tests on memory for words and word frequency.James R. Erickson & Carol Renaud Gaffney - 1985 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23 (4):377-380.
  28.  24
    Effects of orienting tasks and instructions about associative structure on free recall and clustering.Robert E. Till, Carroll D. Johnston & James J. Jenkins - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (4):349-351.
  29.  16
    Optimal behavior in a decision-making task as a function of instructions and payoffs.Gordon F. Pitz & Leslie Downing - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (4p1):549.
  30.  15
    Focusing strategy in concept attainment as a function of instructions and task complexity.Patrick R. Laughlin - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (2):320.
  31.  12
    The relationship of test and general anxiety, difficulty of task, and experimental instructions to performance.Irwin G. Sarason & Ernest G. Palola - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (3):185.
  32.  41
    Focusing in Wason's selection task: Content and instruction effects.Roberta E. Love & Claudius M. Kessler - 1995 - Thinking and Reasoning 1 (2):153 – 182.
  33.  9
    Effects of information load and instructions on performance in a complex task.Paul Stager - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 84 (1):123.
  34.  12
    Total time hypothesis in low-meaningful serial learning: Task, age and verbalization instructions.Elaine C. Koffman & Roy B. Weinstock - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (6):1210.
  35.  14
    Instructed Hand Movements Affect Students’ Learning of an Abstract Concept From Video.Icy Zhang, Karen B. Givvin, Jeffrey M. Sipple, Ji Y. Son & James W. Stigler - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (2):e12940.
    Producing content-related gestures has been found to impact students’ learning, whether such gestures are spontaneously generated by the learner in the course of problem-solving, or participants are instructed to pose based on experimenter instructions during problem-solving and word learning. Few studies, however, have investigated the effect of (a) performing instructed gestures while learning concepts or (b) producing gestures without there being an implied connection between the gestures and the concepts being learned. The two studies reported here investigate the impact (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36.  5
    Knowledge Application and Transfer for Complex Tasks in 111-Structured Domains: Implications for Instruction and Testing in Biomedicine.PaulJ Feltovich, RichardL Coulson, RandJ Spiro & Beth K. Dawson-Saunders - 1992 - In D. A. Evans & V. L. Patel (eds.), Advanced Models of Cognition for Medical Training and Practice. Springer. pp. 213.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  37.  16
    Recall of accessible items from memory as a function of executive instructions, delay tasks, and serial position.Bert Zippel - 1980 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 15 (1):45-47.
  38. Explicit Instructions Do Not Enhance Auditory Statistical Learning in Children With Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence From Event-Related Potentials.Ana Paula Soares, Francisco-Javier Gutiérrez-Domínguez, Helena M. Oliveira, Alexandrina Lages, Natália Guerra, Ana Rita Pereira, David Tomé & Marisa Lousada - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    A current issue in psycholinguistic research is whether the language difficulties exhibited by children with developmental language disorder [DLD, previously labeled specific language impairment ] are due to deficits in their abilities to pick up patterns in the sensory environment, an ability known as statistical learning, and the extent to which explicit learning mechanisms can be used to compensate for those deficits. Studies designed to test the compensatory role of explicit learning mechanisms in children with DLD are, however, scarce, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  11
    Encoding processes for recall and recognition: The effect of instructions and auxiliary task performance.Stephen A. Maisto, Richard J. Dewaard & Marilyn E. Miller - 1977 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 9 (2):127-130.
  40.  31
    Interpreting an intelligent tutor's algorithmic task: A role for apprenticeship as a model for instructional design. [REVIEW]Denis Newman - 1991 - AI and Society 5 (2):93-109.
    The interpretive processes required to understand the context and goals of an algorithmic task are illustrated in the use of an intelligent instructional system developed to train soldiers to monitor a computerized missile's system automatic identification of aircraft. The problems students had in understanding the identification task were addressed in INCOFT, a simulation-based intelligent instructional system that depends, in part, on human instructors to convey the task framework. Supported by recent advances in the cognitive science of instruction, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  31
    Task unrelated thought whilst encoding information.Jonathan M. Smallwood, Simona F. Baracaia, Michelle Lowe & Marc Obonsawin - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (3):452-484.
    Task unrelated thought (TUT) refers to thought directed away from the current situation, for example a daydream. Three experiments were conducted on healthy participants, with two broad aims. First, to contrast distributed and encapsulated views of cognition by comparing the encoding of categorical and random lists of words (Experiments One and Two). Second, to examine the consequences of experiencing TUT during study on the subsequent retrieval of information (Experiments One, Two, and Three). Experiments One and Two demonstrated lower levels (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  42.  18
    Instruction and Practice in Learning to use a Device.Peter A. Bibby & Stephen J. Payne - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (4):539-578.
    We explore the extent to which Anderson's (1987) theory of knowledge compilation can account for the relationship between instructions and practice in learning to use a simple device. Bibby and Payne (1993) reported experimental support for knowledge compilation in this domain. This article replicates the finding of a performance cross‐over between instruction type and task type that disappears with practice on the tasks. The research is extended by using verbal protocols to model the strategies of novice and more (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  43. Situating Instructions.David Kirsh - 2011 - European Perspectives on Cognitive Science.
    A videographic study of origami is presented in which subjects were observed making four different origami objects under five modes of instruction: photos + captions, illustrations-only, illustrations with small captions, illustrations with large captions, and text-only as control. The objective of the study was to explore the gestures and other actions that subjects produce as they try to follow instructions rather than to determine the most effective style of instruction per se. We found that the task of situating (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44. Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction.Camilla K. Gilmore, Shannon E. McCarthy & Elizabeth S. Spelke - unknown
    Symbolic arithmetic is fundamental to science, technology and economics, but its acquisition by children typically requires years of effort, instruction and drill1,2. When adults perform mental arithmetic, they activate nonsymbolic, approximate number representations3,4, and their performance suffers if this nonsymbolic system is impaired5. Nonsymbolic number representations also allow adults, children, and even infants to add or subtract pairs of dot arrays and to compare the resulting sum or difference to a third array, provided that only approximate accuracy is required6–10. Here (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  45. The Knowledge-Learning-Instruction Framework: Bridging the Science-Practice Chasm to Enhance Robust Student Learning.Kenneth R. Koedinger, Albert T. Corbett & Charles Perfetti - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (5):757-798.
    Despite the accumulation of substantial cognitive science research relevant to education, there remains confusion and controversy in the application of research to educational practice. In support of a more systematic approach, we describe the Knowledge-Learning-Instruction (KLI) framework. KLI promotes the emergence of instructional principles of high potential for generality, while explicitly identifying constraints of and opportunities for detailed analysis of the knowledge students may acquire in courses. Drawing on research across domains of science, math, and language learning, we illustrate the (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  46. The Influence of Ethics Instruction, Religiosity, and Intelligence on Cheating Behavior.James M. Bloodgood, William H. Turnley & Peter Mudrack - 2008 - Journal of Business Ethics 82 (3):557-571.
    This study examines the influence of ethics instruction, religiosity, and intelligence on cheating behavior. A sample of 230 upper level, undergraduate business students had the opportunity to increase their chances of winning money in an experimental situation by falsely reporting their task performance. In general, the results indicate that students who attended worship services more frequently were less likely to cheat than those who attended worship services less frequently, but that students who had taken a course in business ethics (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  47. Task unrelated thought whilst encoding information.M. J., F. S., M. Lowe & M. Obonsawin - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (3):452-484.
    Task unrelated thought (TUT) refers to thought directed away from the current situation, for example a daydream. Three experiments were conducted on healthy participants, with two broad aims. First, to contrast distributed and encapsulated views of cognition by comparing the encoding of categorical and random lists of words (Experiments One and Two). Second, to examine the consequences of experiencing TUT during study on the subsequent retrieval of information (Experiments One, Two, and Three). Experiments One and Two demonstrated lower levels (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  48.  44
    Unconscious task set priming with phonological and semantic tasks.Sébastien Weibel, Anne Giersch, Stanislas Dehaene & Caroline Huron - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (2):517-527.
    Whether unconscious stimuli can modulate the preparation of a cognitive task is still controversial. Using a backward masking paradigm, we investigated whether the modulation could be observed even if the prime was made unconscious in 100% of the trials. In two behavioral experiments, subjects were instructed to initiate a phonological or semantic task on an upcoming word, following an explicit instruction and an unconscious prime. When the SOA between prime and instruction was sufficiently long , primes congruent with (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  49.  9
    Task-Irrelevant Context Learned Under Rapid Display Presentation: Selective Attention in Associative Blocking.Xuelian Zang, Leonardo Assumpção, Jiao Wu, Xiaowei Xie & Artyom Zinchenko - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    In the contextual cueing task, visual search is faster for targets embedded in invariant displays compared to targets found in variant displays. However, it has been repeatedly shown that participants do not learn repeated contexts when these are irrelevant to the task. One potential explanation lays in the idea of associative blocking, where salient cues block the learning of invariant associations in the task-irrelevant subset of items. An alternative explanation is that the associative blocking rather hinders the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50. Consciousness and control in task switching.Nachshon Meiran, Bernhard Hommel, Uri Bibi & Idit Lev - 2002 - Consciousness and Cognition 11 (1):10-33.
    Participants were required to switch among randomly ordered tasks, and instructional cues were used to indicate which task to execute. In Experiments 1 and 2, the participants indicated their readiness for the task switch before they received the target stimulus; thus, each trial was associated with two primary dependent measures: (1) readiness time and (2) target reaction time. Slow readiness responses and instructions emphasizing high readiness were paradoxically accompanied by slow target reaction time. Moreover, the effect of (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000