Results for 'hypnotics and sedatives'

921 found
Order:
  1. Managing intentions: The end-of-life administration of analgesics and sedatives, and the possibility of slow euthanasia.Charles Douglas, Ian Kerridge & Rachel Ankeny - 2008 - Bioethics 22 (7):388-396.
    There has been much debate regarding the 'double-effect' of sedatives and analgesics administered at the end-of-life, and the possibility that health professionals using these drugs are performing 'slow euthanasia.' On the one hand analgesics and sedatives can do much to relieve suffering in the terminally ill. On the other hand, they can hasten death. According to a standard view, the administration of analgesics and sedatives amounts to euthanasia when the drugs are given with an intention to hasten (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  2. Sedative-, hypnotic-, or anxiolytic-related disorders. Abuse liability.A. C. Domenic & S. Ofra - forthcoming - Human Studies. In: Kaplan and Sadock’s Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry.
  3.  6
    The image in the text: methodological aspects of the analysis of illustrations and their relation to the text.Gabrielle Sed-Rajna - 1993 - Bulletin of the John Rylands Library 75 (3):25-32.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  16
    Clinical ethics case consultation in a university department of cardiology and intensive care: a descriptive evaluation of consultation protocols.Michel Noutsias, Daniel Sedding, Jochen Dutzmann, Henning Rosenau, Kim P. Linoh, Nicolas Heirich, Stephan Nadolny, Jan Schildmann & Andre Nowak - 2021 - BMC Medical Ethics 22 (1):1-12.
    BackgroundClinical ethics case consultations (CECCs) provide a structured approach in situations of ethical uncertainty or conflicts. There have been increasing calls in recent years to assess the quality of CECCs by means of empirical research. This study provides detailed data of a descriptive quantitative and qualitative evaluation of a CECC service in a department of cardiology and intensive care at a German university hospital.MethodsSemi-structured document analysis of CECCs was conducted in the period of November 1, 2018, to May 31, 2020. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  5.  16
    General practitioners' preferences for managing insomnia and opportunities for reducing hypnotic prescribing.A. Niroshan Siriwardena, Tanefa Apekey, Michelle Tilling, Jane V. Dyas, Hugh Middleton & Roderick Ørner - 2010 - Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice 16 (4):731-737.
  6.  31
    Book review: Aesthetics in feminist perspective. [REVIEW]Hilde Sed Hein & ed Korsmeyer, Carolyn - 1995 - Philosophy and Literature 19 (1).
  7. Narratives of 'terminal sedation', and the importance of the intention-foresight distinction in palliative care practice.Charles D. Douglas, Ian H. Kerridge & Rachel A. Ankeny - 2011 - Bioethics 27 (1):1-11.
    The moral importance of the ‘intention–foresight’ distinction has long been a matter of philosophical controversy, particularly in the context of end-of-life care. Previous empirical research in Australia has suggested that general physicians and surgeons may use analgesic or sedative infusions with ambiguous intentions, their actions sometimes approximating ‘slow euthanasia’. In this paper, we report findings from a qualitative study of 18 Australian palliative care medical specialists, using in-depth interviews to address the use of sedation at the end of life. The (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  8.  9
    Hypnotic and non-hypnotic suggestion to ignore pre-cues decreases space-valence congruency effects in highly hypnotizable individuals.Ya Zhang, Yan Wang & Yixuan Ku - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 65:293-303.
  9.  17
    The neural correlates of movement intentions: A pilot study comparing hypnotic and simulated paralysis.Vera U. Ludwig, Jochen Seitz, Carlos Schönfeldt-Lecuona, Annett Höse, Birgit Abler, Günter Hole, Rainer Goebel & Henrik Walter - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 35:158-170.
  10.  96
    Hypnotic behavior: A social-psychological interpretation of amnesia, analgesia, and “trance logic”.Nicholas P. Spanos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):449-467.
    This paper examines research on three hypnotic phenomena: suggested amnesia, suggested analgesia, and “trance logic.” For each case a social-psychological interpretation of hypnotic behavior as a voluntary response strategy is compared with the traditional special-process view that “good” hypnotic subjects have lost conscious control over suggestion-induced behavior. I conclude that it is inaccurate to describe hypnotically amnesic subjects as unable to recall the material they have been instructed to forget. Although amnesics present themselves as unable to remember, they in fact (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   128 citations  
  11. Hypnotic suggestibility, cognitive inhibition, and dissociation.Zoltán Dienes, Elizabeth Brown, Sam Hutton, Irving Kirsch, Giuliana Mazzoni & Daniel B. Wright - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (4):837-847.
    We examined two potential correlates of hypnotic suggestibility: dissociation and cognitive inhibition. Dissociation is the foundation of two of the major theories of hypnosis and other theories commonly postulate that hypnotic responding is a result of attentional abilities . Participants were administered the Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C. Under the guise of an unrelated study, 180 of these participants also completed: a version of the Dissociative Experiences Scale that is normally distributed in non-clinical populations; a latent inhibition (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  12.  37
    Hypnotic susceptibility, baseline attentional functioning, and the Stroop task.Sandro Rubichi, Federico Ricci, Roberto Padovani & Lorenzo Scaglietti - 2005 - Consciousness and Cognition 14 (2):296-303.
    According to the theoretical framework relating hypnosis to attention, baseline attentional functioning in highly hypnotizable individuals should be more efficient than in low hypnotizable individuals. However, previous studies did not find differences in Stroop-like tasks in which the measure indicative of the Stroop interference effect was based on response latencies. This study was designed to determine whether subjects with different levels of hypnotic susceptibility show differences in baseline attentional functioning. To assess this hypothesis, high, medium, and low hypnotizable subjects performed (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  13.  5
    Hypnotic State Modulates Sensorimotor Beta Rhythms During Real Movement and Motor Imagery.Sébastien Rimbert, Manuel Zaepffel, Pierre Riff, Perrine Adam & Laurent Bougrain - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:478341.
    The hypnosis technique is currently used in the medical field and influences directly the patient's state of relaxation, perception of the body and its visual imagination. There is evidence to suggest that hypnosis state may help patients to better achieve the task of motor imagination, which is central in rehabilitation protocols after a stroke. However, the hypnosis technique could also alter the activity in motor cortex. To the best of our knowledge, the impact of hypnosis on the EEG signal during (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14. Hypnotic phenomena and altered states of consciousness: A multilevel framework of description and explanation.Sakari Kallio & Antti Revonsuo - 2003 - Contemporary Hypnosis 20 (3):111-164.
  15.  30
    Hypnosis, hypnotic suggestibility, memory, and involvement in films.Reed Maxwell, Steven Jay Lynn & Liam Condon - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33:170-184.
  16.  23
    Hypnotic induction is followed by state-like changes in the organization of EEG functional connectivity in the theta and beta frequency bands in high-hypnotically susceptible individuals.Graham A. Jamieson & Adrian P. Burgess - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  17.  43
    Jazz and Musical Works: Hypnotized by the Wrong Model.John Andrew Fisher - 2018 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 76 (2):151-162.
    It is difficult to place jazz within a philosophy of music dominated by the concepts and practices of classical music. One key puzzle concerns the nature and role, if any, of musical works in jazz. I briefly describe the debate between those who deny that there are musical works in jazz (Kania) and those who affirm that there are such (Dodd and others). I argue that musical works are performed in jazz but that jazz performance of works is very different (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  18.  12
    Hypnotic suggestion and the conditioned reflex.V. E. Fisher - 1932 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 15 (2):212.
  19.  48
    Hypnotic responding and self-deception.Irving Kirsch - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (1):118-119.
    As understood by neodissociation and sociocognitive theorists, hypnotic responses are instances of self-deception. Neodissociation theory matches the strict definition of Sackeim and Gur (1978) and sociocognitive theory matches Mele's looser definition. Recent data indicate that many hypnotized individuals deceive themselves into holding conflicting beliefs without dissociating, but others convince themselves that the suggested state of affairs is true without simultaneously holding a contrary belief.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  37
    Hypnotic experience and the autism spectrum disorder. A phenomenological investigation.Till Grohmann - 2018 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 17 (5):889-909.
    In recent decades, the focus in autism research progressively expanded. It presently offers extensive material on sensorimotor disturbances as well as on perceptive-cognitive preferences of people with autism. The present article proposes not only a critical interpretation of the common theoretical framework in autism research but also focuses on certain experiences common to some people with autism and which can be appropriately understood by phenomenology. What I will call “hypnotic experiences” in autism are moments in which some individuals withdraw into (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  17
    Hypnotic regulation of consciousness and the pain neuromatrix.Marie-Elisabeth Faymonville Mélanie Boly, A. Vogt Brent & Steven Laureys Pierre Maquet - 2007 - In Graham A. Jamieson (ed.), Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Oxford University Press. pp. 15-27.
  22.  59
    Metaverse, SED Model, and New Theory of Value.Jianguo Wang, Tongsan Wang, Yuna Shi, Diwei Xu, Yutian Chen & Jie Wu - 2022 - Complexity 2022:1-26.
    The metaverse concept constructs a virtual world parallel to the real world. The social economic dynamics model establishes a systematic model for social economic dynamics simulation that integrates macroeconomy and microeconomy based on modeling mechanism of the new theory of value by analogy with Newtonian mechanics and the modeling approach of Agent-based computational economics. This article describes the SED model’s modeling mechanisms, modeling rules, and behavior equations. At the same time, this article introduces the methods, testing standards, and some typical (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  20
    Hypnotic recall of material learned under anxiety- and non-anxiety-producing conditions.B. G. Rosenthal - 1944 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 34 (5):369.
  24.  11
    Hypnotic susceptibility, EEG-alpha, and self-regulation.David R. Engstrom - 1976 - In Gary E. Schwartz & D. H. Shapiro (eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation. Plenum. pp. 173--221.
  25.  32
    Altered and asymmetric default mode network activity in a “hypnotic virtuoso”: An fMRI and EEG study.S. Lipari, F. Baglio, L. Griffanti, L. Mendozzi, M. Garegnani, A. Motta, P. Cecconi & L. Pugnetti - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (1):393-400.
  26.  6
    Unique effects of sedatives, dissociatives, psychedelics, stimulants, and cannabinoids on episodic memory: A review and reanalysis of acute drug effects on recollection, familiarity, and metamemory.Manoj K. Doss, Jason Samaha, Frederick S. Barrett, Roland R. Griffiths, Harriet de Wit, David A. Gallo & Joshua D. Koen - 2024 - Psychological Review 131 (2):523-562.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  4
    Discussion and reports: Post-hypnotic suggestion and determinism.H. H. Schroeder - 1902 - Psychological Review 9 (3):283-292.
  28.  27
    The Hypnotic Phenomena and Religious Experience.Hiroshi Motoyama - 1968 - Zeitschrift für Religions- Und Geistesgeschichte 20 (4):321-339.
  29.  22
    Lack of correlation between hypnotic susceptibility and various components of attention.Katalin Varga, Zoltán Németh & Anna Szekely - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1872-1881.
    The purpose of our study was to measure the relationship between performance on various attentional tasks and hypnotic susceptibility. Healthy volunteers participated in a study, where they had to perform several tasks measuring various attention components in a waking state: sustained attention, selective or focused attention, divided attention and executive attention in task switching. Hypnotic susceptibility was measured in a separate setting by the Waterloo-Stanford Groups Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C .We found no significant correlation between any of the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  30. Hypnotic regulation of consciousness and the pain neuromatrix.Melanie Boly, Marie-Elisabeth Faymonville, Brent A. Vogt, Pierre Maquet & Laureys & Steven - 2007 - In Graham Jamieson (ed.), Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Oxford University Press.
  31.  19
    Social and psychological influences on hypnotic behavior.Campbell Perry & Jean-Roch Laurence - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):478-479.
  32.  88
    Post-hypnotic suggestion and the existence of unconscious mental activity.Donald Levy - 1983 - Analysis 43 (4):184.
  33. Sedative and amnesic effects of mirfentanil.R. C. Cork, T. H. Kramer & J. F. Kihlstrom - 1993 - In P. S. Sebel, B. Bonke & E. Winograd (eds.), Memory and Awareness in Anesthesia. Prentice-Hall. pp. 141.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  6
    Errors and Action Monitoring: Errare Humanum Est Sed Corrigere Possibile.Franck Vidal, Boris Burle & Thierry Hasbroucq - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  35.  46
    Hypnosis and belief: A review of hypnotic delusions. [REVIEW]Michael H. Connors - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 36:27-43.
  36.  70
    Hypnotizing Libet: Readiness potentials with non-conscious volition.Alexander Schlegel, Prescott Alexander, Walter Sinnott-Armstrong, Adina Roskies, Peter Ulric Tse & Thalia Wheatley - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33 (C):196-203.
    The readiness potential (RP) is one of the most controversial topics in neuroscience and philosophy due to its perceived relevance to the role of conscious willing in action. Libet and colleagues reported that RP onset precedes both volitional movement and conscious awareness of willing that movement, suggesting that the experience of conscious will may not cause volitional movement (Libet, Gleason, Wright, & Pearl, 1983). Rather, they suggested that the RP indexes unconscious processes that may actually cause both volitional movement and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  37.  46
    Not all group hypnotic suggestibility scales are created equal: Individual differences in behavioral and subjective responses☆.Sean M. Barnes, Steven Jay Lynn & Ronald J. Pekala - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (1):255-265.
    To examine the influence of hypnotic suggestibility testing as a source of individual differences in hypnotic responsiveness, we compared behavioral and subjective responses on three scales of hypnotic suggestibility: The Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A . Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility. Berlin: Consulting Psychologists Press); the Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale . The Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale: Normative data and psychometric properties. Psychological Reports, 53, 523–535); and the Group Scale of Hypnotic Ability . (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  38.  30
    Continuous deep sedation and the doctrine of double effect: Do physicians not intend to make the patient unconscious until death if they gradually increase the sedatives?Hitoshi Arima - 2020 - Bioethics 34 (9):977-983.
    Continuous deep sedation (CDS) has the effect of making the patient unconscious until death, and that it has this effect is clearly an undesirable aspect of CDS. However, some authors have recently maintained that many physicians do not intend this effect when practicing CDS. According to these authors, CDS is differentiated into two types; in what is called “gradual” CDS (or CDS as a result of proportionate palliative sedation), physicians start with low doses of sedatives and increase them only (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  54
    Impulsivity, self-control, and hypnotic suggestibility.V. U. Ludwig, C. Stelzel, H. Krutiak, C. E. Prunkl, R. Steimke, L. M. Paschke, N. Kathmann & H. Walter - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (2):637-653.
    Hypnotic responding might be due to attenuated frontal lobe functioning after the hypnotic induction. Little is known about whether personality traits linked with frontal functioning are associated with responsiveness to hypnotic suggestions. We assessed whether hypnotic suggestibility is related to the traits of self-control and impulsivity in 154 participants who completed the Brief Self-Control Scale, the Self-Regulation Scale, the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale , and the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility . BIS-11 non-planning impulsivity correlated positively with HGSHS:A . Furthermore, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  40.  17
    Hypnotic behavior dissected or … pulling the wings off butterflies.Dennis C. Turk & Thomas E. Rudy - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):485-485.
  41.  35
    Hypnotic control of attention in the stroop task: A historical footnote.Colin M. MacLeod & Peter W. Sheehan - 2003 - Consciousness and Cognition 12 (3):347-353.
    have recently provided a compelling demonstration of enhanced attentional control under post-hypnotic suggestion. Using the classic color-word interference paradigm, in which the task is to ignore a word and to name the color in which it is printed (e.g., RED in green, say ''green''), they gave a post-hypnotic instruction to participants that they would be unable to read. This eliminated Stroop interference in high suggestibility participants but did not alter interference in low suggestibility participants. replicated this pattern and further demonstrated (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  42.  50
    Hypnotic induction decreases anterior default mode activity.William J. McGeown, Giuliana Mazzoni, Annalena Venneri & Irving Kirsch - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (4):848-855.
    The ‘default mode’ network refers to cortical areas that are active in the absence of goal-directed activity. In previous studies, decreased activity in the ‘default mode’ has always been associated with increased activation in task-relevant areas. We show that the induction of hypnosis can reduce anterior default mode activity during rest without increasing activity in other cortical regions. We assessed brain activation patterns of high and low suggestible people while resting in the fMRI scanner and while engaged in visual tasks, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  43.  20
    Phase-ordered gamma oscillations and the modulation of hypnotic experience.Vilfredo De Pascalis - 2007 - In Graham A. Jamieson (ed.), Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Oxford University Press. pp. 67-89.
  44.  13
    The influence of repetition and disuse upon rate of hypnotization.R. G. Krueger - 1931 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 14 (3):260.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  45. Reaction-time and attention in the hypnotic state.G. Stanley Hall - 1883 - Mind 8 (30):170-182.
  46.  73
    Hypnotic suggestibility predicts the magnitude of the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect in a non-hypnotic context.Benjamin A. Parris & Zoltan Dienes - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (3):868-874.
    The present study investigated how the magnitude the word blindness suggestion effect on Stroop interference depended on hypnotic suggestibility when given as an imaginative suggestion and under conditions in which hypnosis was not mentioned. Hypnotic suggestibility is shown to be a significant predictor of the magnitude of the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect under these conditions. This is therefore the first study to show a linear relationship between the imaginative word blindness suggestion effect and hypnotic suggestibility across the whole hypnotizability (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  47.  15
    Comparative effects of hypnotic suggestion and imagery instruction on bodily awareness.C. Apelian, F. De Vignemont & D. B. Terhune - 2023 - Consciousness and Cognition 108 (C):103473.
  48.  55
    Is hypnotic suggestibility a stable trait?☆.Oliver Fassler, Steven Jay Lynn & Joshua Knox - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):240-253.
    The present study examined the trait-like nature of hypnotic suggestibility by examining the stability of hypnotic responsiveness in a test–retest design in which the procedures were administered either live or by audiotape. Contrary to the idea that hypnotizability is a largely immutable, stable trait, scores on the scale of hypnotic responsiveness decreased significantly at the second session. Measures of subjective experiences and expectancies accounted for a sizable portion of the variance in hypnotic responding, both at initial test and at retest. (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  49.  20
    Dissociated control and the limits of hypnotic responsiveness.Kenneth S. Bowers - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (1):32-39.
  50.  5
    "...Sed intelligere": studi in onore di Franco Biasutti.Romana Bassi, Carla Ravazzolo, Gabriele Tomasi & Franco Biasutti (eds.) - 2022 - Padova: CLEUP.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 921