Results for 'egocentric spatial representation'

986 found
Order:
  1. Egocentric Spatial Representation in Action and Perception.Robert Briscoe - 2009 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 79 (2):423-460.
    Neuropsychological findings used to motivate the "two visual systems" hypothesis have been taken to endanger a pair of widely accepted claims about spatial representation in conscious visual experience. The first is the claim that visual experience represents 3-D space around the perceiver using an egocentric frame of reference. The second is the claim that there is a constitutive link between the spatial contents of visual experience and the perceiver's bodily actions. In this paper, I review and (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  2.  45
    Being Somewhere. Egocentic spatial representation as self-representation.Ferdinand Pöhlmann - 2017 - Stuttgart: J.B. Metzler.
    Ferdinand Pöhlmann argues that a sense of one’s own basic abilities to move is a constitutive condition on the ability to perceive the world spatially. This constitutive relation explains why egocentric spatial representation is to be regarded as a kind of self-representation. In arguing for these claims, conceptual as well as empirical questions are discussed and an overview of accounts that take action as a constitutive condition on spatial representation is given. The picture that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  46
    The complex interplay between three-dimensional egocentric and allocentric spatial representation.David M. Kaplan - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):553-554.
    Jeffery et al. characterize the egocentric/allocentric distinction as discrete. But paradoxically, much of the neural and behavioral evidence they adduce undermines a discrete distinction. More strikingly, their positive proposal reflects a more complex interplay between egocentric and allocentric coding than they acknowledge. Properly interpreted, their proposal about three-dimensional spatial representation contributes to recent work on embodied cognition.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  66
    Conceptuality in spatial representations.Gottfried Vosgerau - 2007 - Philosophical Psychology 20 (3):349 – 365.
    The notion of conceptuality is still unclear and vague. I will present a definition of conceptual and nonconceptual representations that is grounded in different aspects of the representations’ structures. This definition is then used to interpret empirical results from human and animal navigation. It will be shown, that the distinction between egocentric and allocentric spatial representations can be matched onto the conceptual vs. nonconceptual distinction. The phenomena discussed in spatial navigation are thereby put into a wider context (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  5. Self, world and space: The meaning and mechanisms of ego- and allocentric spatial representation.Rick Grush - 2000 - Brain and Mind 1 (1):59-92.
    b>: The problem of how physical systems, such as brains, come to represent themselves as subjects in an objective world is addressed. I develop an account of the requirements for this ability that draws on and refines work in a philosophical tradition that runs from Kant through Peter Strawson to Gareth Evans. The basic idea is that the ability to represent oneself as a subject in a world whose existence is independent of oneself involves the ability to represent space, and (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  6. Unilateral neglect and the objectivity of spatial representation.Bill Brewer - 1992 - Mind and Language 7 (3):222-39.
    Patients may show a more-or-less complete deviation of the head and eyes towards the right (ipsilesional) side [that is, to the same side of egocentric space as the brain lesion responsible for their disorder]. If addressed by the examiner from the left (contralesional) side [the opposite side to their lesion], patients with severe extrapersonal neglect may fail to respond or may look for the speaker in the right side of the room, turning head and eyes more and more to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  7.  9
    Children’s use of egocentric reference frames in spatial language is related to their numerical magnitude understanding.Nadja Lindner, Korbinian Moeller, Frauke Hildebrandt, Marcus Hasselhorn & Jan Lonnemann - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Numerical magnitude information is assumed to be spatially represented in the form of a mental number line defined with respect to a body-centred, egocentric frame of reference. In this context, spatial language skills such as mastery of verbal descriptions of spatial position have been proposed to be relevant for grasping spatial relations between numerical magnitudes on the mental number line. We examined 4- to 5-year-old’s spatial language skills in tasks that allow responses in egocentric (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8. Egocentric Space.Joel Smith - 2014 - International Journal of Philosophical Studies 22 (3):409-433.
    I discuss the relation between egocentric spatial representation and the capacity for bodily activity, with specific reference to Merleau-Ponty.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  9. Spatial Content and Motoric Significance.Robert Briscoe - 2014 - Avant: Trends in Interdisciplinary Studies 1 (2):199-216.
    According to “actionism” (Noë 2010), perception constitutively depends on implicit knowledge of the way sensory stimulations vary as a consequence of the perceiver’s self-movement. My aim in this contribution is to develop an alternative conception of the role of action in perception present in the work of Gareth Evans using resources provided by Ruth Millikan’s biosemantic theory of mental representation.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  10.  21
    Social and Representational Cues Jointly Influence Spatial Perspective‐Taking.Alexia Galati & Marios N. Avraamides - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (4):739-765.
    We examined how social cues and representational ones jointly shape people's spatial memory representations and their subsequent descriptions. In 24 pairs, Directors studied an array with a symmetrical structure while either knowing their Matcher's subsequent viewpoint or not. During the subsequent description of the array, the array's intrinsic structure was aligned with the Director, the Matcher, or neither partner. According to memory tests preceding descriptions, Directors who had studied the array while aligned with its structure were more likely to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  11. Bodily spatial content.Frederique de Vignemont - 2009 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 15 (1).
    The classic notion of an egocentric frame of reference cannot be easily applied to bodily space, given the difficulties in providing a centre of such frame as well as axes on which one could compute distances and directions . Yet, Smith tries to rehabilitate the egocentric account of bodily frame by switching from an anatomical definition of egocentricity to a more functional definition . Here I will review some empirical evidence that shows that one cannot ground bodily experiences (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  12.  52
    Explaining “spatial purport of perception”: a predictive processing approach.Wiktor Rorot - 2020 - Synthese 198 (10):9739-9762.
    Despite the large interest in the human ability to perceive space present in neuroscience, cognitive science and psychology, as well as philosophy of mind, the issues regarding egocentric space representation received relatively less attention. In this paper I take up a unique phenomenon related to this faculty: the “spatial purport” of perceptual experiences. The notion was proposed by Rick Grush to describe the subjective, qualitative aspects of egocentric representations of spatial properties and relations. Although Grush (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  50
    Stepping Into a Map: Initial Heading Direction Influences Spatial Memory Flexibility.Stephanie A. Gagnon, Tad T. Brunyé, Aaron Gardony, Matthijs L. Noordzij, Caroline R. Mahoney & Holly A. Taylor - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (2):275-302.
    Learning a novel environment involves integrating first-person perceptual and motoric experiences with developing knowledge about the overall structure of the surroundings. The present experiments provide insights into the parallel development of these egocentric and allocentric memories by intentionally conflicting body- and world-centered frames of reference during learning, and measuring outcomes via online and offline measures. Results of two experiments demonstrate faster learning and increased memory flexibility following route perspective reading (Experiment 1) and virtual navigation (Experiment 2) when participants begin (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  14. Spatial representation: problems in philosophy and psychology.Naomi Eilan, Rosaleen A. McCarthy & Bill Brewer (eds.) - 1993 - Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell.
    Spatial Representation presents original, specially written essays by leading psychologists and philosophers on a fascinating set of topics at the intersection of these two disciplines. They address such questions as these: Do the extraordinary navigational abilities of birds mean that these birds have the same kind of grip on the idea of a spatial world as we do? Is there a difference between the way sighted and blind subjects represent the world 'out there'? Does the study of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  15.  68
    Spatial representations in sensory modalities.Tony Cheng - 2022 - Mind and Language 37 (3):485-500.
    Some sensory modalities, such as sight, touch and audition, are arguably spatial, and one way to understand these spatial senses is to investigate spatial representations in them. Here I focus on a specific element in this area— the interplay between perspectival variation and spatial constancy—and discuss recent interdisciplinary works on this topic. With these relevant experimental works, we will see clearly how traditional controversies in philosophy, for example, whether we perceive perspectival shapes as well as objective (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  16.  22
    Ongoing egocentric spatial processing during learning of non-spatial information results in temporal-parietal activity during retrieval.Alice Gomez, Mélanie Cerles, Stéphane Rousset, Jean-François Le Bas & Monica Baciu - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  17.  63
    Spatial representations activated during real‐time comprehension of verbs.Daniel C. Richardson, Michael J. Spivey, Lawrence W. Barsalou & Ken McRae - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (5):767-780.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  18.  35
    Husserl’s Lebenswelt and the problem of spatial cognition – in search of universals.Elżbieta Łukasiewicz - 2010 - Polish Journal of Philosophy 4 (1):23-43.
    Perception and conceptualization of space are some of the most basic elements of human cognition. It has been long assumed that human spatial thinkingand frames of reference used to grasp and describe the location of an object in relation to other objects are of universal nature and so are projected in naturallanguages in basically the same manner; three principal dimensions in egocentric perceptual space were distinguished: up-down, front-back and left-right, reflecting our biological make-up. If differences in spatial (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  55
    Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect.E. Rusconi, B. Kwan, B. Giordano, C. Umilta & B. Butterworth - 2006 - Cognition 99 (2):113-129.
  20.  20
    Audio Spatial Representation Around the Body.Aggius-Vella Elena, Campus Claudio, Finocchietti Sara & Gori Monica - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  21. Spatial representation, magnitude and the two stems of cognition.Thomas Land - 2014 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 44 (5-6):524-550.
    The aim of this paper is to show that attention to Kant's philosophy of mathematics sheds light on the doctrine that there are two stems of the cognitive capacity, which are distinct, but equally necessary for cognition. Specifically, I argue for the following four claims: The distinctive structure of outer sensible intuitions must be understood in terms of the concept of magnitude. The act of sensibly representing a magnitude involves a special act of spontaneity Kant ascribes to a capacity he (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  22.  15
    Spatial representation of objects in the young blind child.Barbara Landau - 1991 - Cognition 38 (2):145-178.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  23. Spatial Representation. Problems in philosophy and psychology.Naomi Eilan, Rosaleen Mccarthy & Bill Brewer - 2001 - Revue Philosophique de la France Et de l'Etranger 191 (1):119-120.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  24.  47
    Spatial Representations Elicit Dual‐Coding Effects in Mental Imagery.Michelle Verges & Sean Duffy - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (6):1157-1172.
    Spatial aspects of words are associated with their canonical locations in the real world. Yet little research has tested whether spatial associations denoted in language comprehension generalize to their corresponding images. We directly tested the spatial aspects of mental imagery in picture and word processing (Experiment 1). We also tested whether spatial representations of motion words produce similar perceptual-interference effects as demonstrated by object words (Experiment 2). Findings revealed that words denoting an upward spatial location (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  25. Spatial representation in mind and brain.Michael McCloskey - 2001 - In B. Rapp (ed.), The Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology: What Deficits Reveal About the Human Mind. Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis. pp. 101--132.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  16
    Attention, spatial representation, and visual neglect: Simulating emergent attention and spatial memory in the selective attention for identification model (SAIM).Dietmar Heinke & Glyn W. Humphreys - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (1):29-87.
  27.  12
    Spatial Representations in the Human Brain.Nora A. Herweg & Michael J. Kahana - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  28. Spatial Representation.Ruth G. Millikan - 1993 - Cambridge: Blackwell.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  29. Spatial Representation.John O'Keefe - 1993 - Cambridge: Blackwell.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  30.  35
    Spatial Representation of Ordinal Information.Meng Zhang, Xuefei Gao, Baichen Li, Shuyuan Yu, Tianwei Gong, Ting Jiang, Qingfen Hu & Yinghe Chen - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31.  14
    Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching.A. Pouget - 2002 - Cognition 83 (1):1-11.
  32.  89
    Husserl on Geometry and Spatial Representation.Jairo José da Silva - 2012 - Axiomathes 22 (1):5-30.
    Husserl left many unpublished drafts explaining (or trying to) his views on spatial representation and geometry, such as, particularly, those collected in the second part of Studien zur Arithmetik und Geometrie (Hua XXI), but no completely articulate work on the subject. In this paper, I put forward an interpretation of what those views might have been. Husserl, I claim, distinguished among different conceptions of space, the space of perception (constituted from sensorial data by intentionally motivated psychic functions), that (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  33.  18
    Spatial representations of numbers and letters in children.Jan Lonnemann, Janosch Linkersdörfer, Telse Nagler, Marcus Hasselhorn & Sven Lindberg - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  34. Parts and Places: The Structures of Spatial Representation.Roberto Casati & Achille C. Varzi - 1999 - MIT Press.
    Thinking about space is thinking about spatial things. The table is on the carpet; hence the carpet is under the table. The vase is in the box; hence the box is not in the vase. But what does it mean for an object to be somewhere? How are objects tied to the space they occupy? This book is concerned with these and other fundamental issues in the philosophy of spatial representation. Our starting point is an analysis of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   204 citations  
  35.  28
    Anticipatory spatial representation of 3D regions explored by sighted observers and a deaf-and-blind-observer.Helene Intraub - 2004 - Cognition 94 (1):19-37.
  36.  16
    Multimodal spatial representations in the primate parietal lobe.Yale E. Cohen & Richard A. Andersen - 2004 - In Charles Spence & Jon Driver (eds.), Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention. Oxford University Press. pp. 154--176.
  37. Multimodal spatial representations in the primate parietal lobe.Yale E. Cohen & Andersen & A. Richard - 2004 - In Charles Spence & Jon Driver (eds.), Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention. Oxford University Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  9
    What spatial representation and language acquisition don't have in common.Steven Pinker - 1981 - Cognition 10 (1-3):243-248.
  39.  4
    Idiosyncratic spatial representations of the days of the week in individuals without synesthesia.Shogo Makioka - 2021 - Cognition 207 (C):104500.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40. Spatial representation and reasoning.Jerry R. Hobbs & Srini Narayanan - 2002 - In Lynn Nadel (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Macmillan.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  7
    Spatial representation, activity, and meaning: Children's images of the contemporary city.Kyriaki Tsoukala - 2009 - Semiotica 2009 (175):77-133.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42. Spatial Representation.Michael Tye - 1993 - Cambridge: Blackwell.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43.  15
    Are spatial representations flattish?J. B. Deregowski - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):243-244.
  44. Spatial Representation.Andrew N. Meltzoff - 1993 - Cambridge: Blackwell.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  45.  9
    Spatial Representations and Their Physical Content.Olga Fernandez Prat & Daniel Quesada - 1997 - Poznan Studies in the Philosophy of the Sciences and the Humanities 61:107-134.
  46.  12
    Central spatial representations and mapping the sensorimotor interface: How early is early, how late is late, and what difference does it all make anyhow?Paul Grobstein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):339-341.
  47.  48
    Husserl on Geometry and Spatial Representation.Jairo José Silva - 2012 - Axiomathes 22 (1):5-30.
    Husserl left many unpublished drafts explaining (or trying to) his views on spatial representation and geometry, such as, particularly, those collected in the second part of Studien zur Arithmetik und Geometrie (Hua XXI), but no completely articulate work on the subject. In this paper, I put forward an interpretation of what those views might have been. Husserl, I claim, distinguished among different conceptions of space, the space of perception (constituted from sensorial data by intentionally motivated psychic functions), that (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  48.  21
    The Relational SNARC: Spatial Representation of Nonsymbolic Ratios.Rui Meng, Percival G. Matthews & Elizabeth Y. Toomarian - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (8):e12778.
    Recent research in numerical cognition has begun to systematically detail the ability of humans and nonhuman animals to perceive the magnitudes of nonsymbolic ratios. These relationally defined analogs to rational numbers offer new potential insights into the nature of human numerical processing. However, research into their similarities with and connections to symbolic numbers remains in its infancy. The current research aims to further explore these similarities by investigating whether the magnitudes of nonsymbolic ratios are associated with space just as symbolic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  49. Moderate Conceptualism and Spatial Representation.Thomas Land - 2016 - In Dennis Schulting (ed.), Kantian Nonconceptualism. London, United Kingdom: Palgrave Macmillan. pp. 145-170.
    In this paper I argue that Kant’s theory of spatial representation supports a Moderate Conceptualist view of his theory of intuition, according to which Kantian intuitions depend for their objective purport on actualizations of spontaneity in a particular kind of synthesis. In making the case for this I focus on three aspects of the theory of spatial representation: the distinction Kant draws between what he calls the original representation of space and the representations of determinate (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  50. Spatial language and spatial representation: a cross-linguistic comparison.Edward Munnich, Barbara Landau & Barbara Anne Dosher - 2001 - Cognition 81 (3):171-208.
1 — 50 / 986