12 found
Order:
  1.  35
    Do people differentially remember cheaters?Pat Barclay & Martin L. Lalumière - 2006 - Human Nature 17 (1):98-113.
    The evolution of reciprocal altruism probably involved the evolution of mechanisms to detect cheating and remember cheaters. In a well-known study, Mealey, Daood, and Krage (1996) observed that participants had enhanced memory for faces that had previously been associated with descriptions of acts of cheating. There were, however, problems with the descriptions that were used in that study. We sought to replicate and extend the findings of Mealey and colleagues by using more controlled descriptions and by examining the possibility of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  2.  25
    Enhanced recognition of defectors depends on their rarity.Pat Barclay - 2008 - Cognition 107 (3):817-828.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  3.  75
    Proximate and ultimate causes of punishment and strong reciprocity.Pat Barclay & Francesco Guala - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (1):16.
    While admirable, Guala's discussion of reciprocity suffers from a confusion between proximate causes (psychological mechanisms triggering behaviour) and ultimate causes (evolved function of those psychological mechanisms). Because much work on commits this error, I clarify the difference between proximate and ultimate causes of cooperation and punishment. I also caution against hasty rejections of of experimental evidence.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  4.  14
    Attractiveness biases are the tip of the iceberg in biological markets.Pat Barclay - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    Physical attractiveness affects how one gets treated, but it is just a single component of one's overall “market value.” One's treatment depends on other markers of market value, including social status, competence, warmth, and any other cues of one's ability or willingness to confer benefits on partners. To completely understand biased treatment, we must also incorporate these other factors.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  9
    Fearful apes or emotional cooperative breeders?Pat Barclay, Savannah Yerman & Oliver Twardus - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e53.
    The “fearful ape hypothesis” is interesting but is currently underspecified. We need more research on whether it is specific to fear, specific to humans (or even cooperative breeders in general), what is included in “fear,” and whether these patterns would indeed evolve despite arms races to extract help from audiences. Specifying these will result in a more testable hypothesis.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  48
    Humans should be individualistic and utility-maximizing, but not necessarily “rational”.Pat Barclay & Martin Daly - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (2):154-155.
    One reason why humans don't behave according to standard game theoretical rationality is because it's not realistic to assume that everyone else is behaving rationally. An individual is expected to have psychological mechanisms that function to maximize his/her long-term payoffs in a world of potentially “irrational” individuals. Psychological decision theory has to be individualistic because individuals make decisions, not groups.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  40
    Pathways to abnormal revenge and forgiveness.Pat Barclay - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (1):17-18.
    The target article's important point is easily misunderstood to claim that all revenge is adaptive. Revenge and forgiveness can overstretch the bounds of utility due to misperceptions, minimization of costly errors, a breakdown within our evolved revenge systems, or natural genetic and developmental variation. Together, these factors can compound to produce highly abnormal instances of revenge and forgiveness.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  13
    The burden of proof for a cultural group selection account.Pat Barclay & Daniel Brian Krupp - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  11
    Broadening the role of “self-interest” in folk-economic beliefs.Mia Karabegović, Amanda Rotella & Pat Barclay - 2018 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 41.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  40
    Let's add some psychology (and maybe even some evolution) to the mix.Daniel Brian Krupp, Pat Barclay, Martin Daly, Toko Kiyonari, Greg Dingle & Margo Wilson - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (6):828-829.
    Henrich et al.'s nice cross-cultural experiments would benefit from models that specify the decision rules that humans use and the specific developmental pathways that allow cooperative norms to be internalized. Such models could help researchers to design further experiments to examine human social adaptations. We must also test whether the “same” experiments measure similar constructs in each culture, using additional methods and measures.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  11.  3
    Feelings of obligation are valuations of signaling-mediated social payoffs.Amanda Rotella, Adam Maxwell Sparks & Pat Barclay - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43.
    We extend Tomasello's framework by addressing the functional challenge of obligation. If the long-run social consequences of a decision are sufficiently costly, obligation motivates the actor to forgo potential immediate benefits in favor of long-term social interests. Thus, obligation psychology balances the downstream socially-mediated payoffs from a decision. This perspective can predict when and why obligation will be experienced.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  46
    Fundamental freedoms and the psychology of threat, bargaining, and inequality.Adam Sparks, Sandeep Mishra & Pat Barclay - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):500-501.
    Van de Vliert's findings may be explained by the psychology of threat and bargaining. Poor people facing extreme threats must cope by surrendering individual freedom in service of shared group needs. Wealthier people are more able to flee from threats and/or resist authoritarianism, so their leaders must concede greater freedom. Incorporating these factors (plus inequality) can sharpen researchers' predictions.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark