Results for 'Genetic similarity theory'

981 found
Order:
  1.  9
    Genetic similarity theory needs more development.R. I. M. Dunbar - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):520-521.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  6
    Clinical ethical practice and associated factors in healthcare facilities in Ethiopia: a cross-sectional study.Nebiyou Tafesse, Assegid Samuel, Abiyu Geta, Fantanesh Desalegn, Lidia Gebru, Tezera Tadele, Ewnetu Genet, Mulugeta Abate & Kemal Jemal - 2022 - BMC Medical Ethics 23 (1):1-12.
    BackgroundClinical ethical practice (CEP) is required for healthcare workers (HCWs) to improve health-care delivery. However, there are gaps between accepted ethical standards and CEP in Ethiopia. There have been limited studies conducted on CEP in the country. Therefore, this study aimed to determine the magnitude and associated factors of CEP among healthcare workers in healthcare facilities in Ethiopia.MethodFrom February to April 2021, a mixed-method study was conducted in 24 health facilities, combining quantitative and qualitative methods. Quantitative (survey questionnaire) and qualitative (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  8
    Biocultural versus biological systems: Implications for genetic similarity theory.C. Scott Findlay - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):524-525.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  52
    Similarity Arguments in the Genetic Modification Debate.Andreas Christiansen - 2017 - Ethical Theory and Moral Practice 20 (2):239-255.
    In the ethical debate on genetic modification, it is common to encounter the claim that some anti-GM argument would also apply an established, ethically accepted technology, and that the anti-GM argument is therefore unsuccessful. The paper discusses whether this argumentative strategy, the Similarity Argument, is sound. It presents a logically valid, generic form of the Similarity Argument and then shows that it is subject to three types of objection: It does not respect the difference between pro tanto (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  29
    A few comments on electrostatic interactions in cell physiology.Stéphane Genet, Robert Costalat & Jacques Burger - 2000 - Acta Biotheoretica 48 (3-4):273-287.
    The role of fixed charges present at the surface of biological membranes is usually described by the Gouy-Chapman-Grahame theory of the electric double-layer where the Grahame equation is applied independently on each side of the membrane and where the capacitive charges are disregarded. In this article, we generalize the Gouy-Chapman-Grahame theory by taking into account both intrinsic charges and capacitive charges, in the density value of the membrane surface charges. In the first part, we show that capacitive charges (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  39
    The Theory Of Social–Cultural Genetic Gene (S- c Dna).Jiayin Min - 2013 - World Futures 69 (2):89 - 101.
    It took a long time for humanity to know about biogenetics. And yet its role as a determinant in the living system was not proven until the twentieth century when DNA was discovered. Similarly, it took a long time for humanity to know about culture and civilization. And yet until now there is neither definite standards for differentiating them nor a definition that is commonly acceptable. By taking an evolutionary pluralism as ontology framework and the transdisciplinary research method of the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  43
    Is symbolic inheritance similar to genetic inheritance?Luc Steels - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (4):376-377.
    Jablonka & Lamb's (J&L's) book is refreshing in that it debunks the exclusively gene-centered approach used these days to explain almost anything about life and human behavior. The book is very accessible and most convincing when the authors discuss biological theories of genetic and epigenetic inheritance, but it does not shy away from the more slippery terrain of behavioral and symbolic inheritance, and specifically the origins of language. But is the analogy appropriate?
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  19
    The Uroboros Theory of Life’s Origin: 22-Nucleotide Theoretical Minimal RNA Rings Reflect Evolution of Genetic Code and tRNA-rRNA Translation Machineries.Jacques Demongeot & Hervé Seligmann - 2019 - Acta Biotheoretica 67 (4):273-297.
    Theoretical minimal RNA rings attempt to mimick life’s primitive RNAs. At most 25 22-nucleotide-long RNA rings code once for each biotic amino acid, a start and a stop codon and form a stem-loop hairpin, resembling consensus tRNAs. We calculated, for each RNA ring’s 22 potential splicing positions, similarities of predicted secondary structures with tRNA vs. rRNA secondary structures. Assuming rRNAs partly derived from tRNA accretions, we predict positive associations between relative secondary structure similarities with rRNAs over tRNAs and genetic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  9. The genetic recombination of science and religion.Stephen M. Modell - 2010 - Zygon 45 (2):462-468.
    The estrangement between genetic scientists and theologians originating in the 1960s is reflected in novel combinations of human thought (subject) and genes (investigational object), paralleling each other through the universal process known in chaos theory as self-similarity. The clash and recombination of genes and knowledge captures what Philip Hefner refers to as irony, one of four voices he suggests transmit the knowledge and arguments of the religion-and-science debate. When viewed along a tangent connecting irony to leadership, journal (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10.  10
    Molecular genetics of aging in the fly: Is this the end of the beginning?Stephen L. Helfand & Blanka Rogina - 2003 - Bioessays 25 (2):134-141.
    How we age and what we can do about it have been uppermost in human thought since antiquity. The many false starts have frustrated experimentalists and theoretical arguments pronouncing the inevitability of the process have created a nihilistic climate among scientists and the public. The identification of single gene alterations that substantially extend life span in nematodes and flies however, have begun to reinvigorate the field. Drosophila's long history of contributions to aging research, rich storehouse of genetic information, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  10
    Structuralism and Form in Literature and Biology: Critiquing Genetic Manipulation.Peter McMahon - 2024 - Springer Nature Switzerland.
    The book considers biology in parallel with philosophical structuralism in order to argue that notions of form in the organism are analogous to similar ideas in structuralist philosophy and literary theory. This analogy is then used to shed light on debates among biological scientists from the turn of the 19th century to the present day, including Cuvier, Geoffroy Saint-Hilaire, Dawkins, Crick, Goodwin, Rosen and West-Eberhard. The book critiques the endorsement of genetic manipulation and bioengineering as keys to solving (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  30
    The influence of initial attitudes on responses to communication about genetic engineering in food production.Lynn J. Frewer, Chaya Howard & Richard Shepherd - 1998 - Agriculture and Human Values 15 (1):15-30.
    Source credibility has been thought to bean important determinant of peoples‘ reactions toinformation about technology. There has also been muchdebate about the need to communicate effectively withthe public about genetic engineering, particularlywithin the context of food production. Questionnaireswere used to investigate the impact of sourcecredibility, admission of risk uncertainty, andinitial attitude towards genetic engineering onattitudes of respondents after information provision.120 respondents with positive attitudes towardsgenetic engineering in food production were providedwith persuasive information about the technology,where both source attribution (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  13.  68
    Gilbert Simondon’s genetic “mecanology”and the understanding of laws of technical evolution.Vincent Bontems - 2009 - Techné: Research in Philosophy and Technology 13 (1):1-12.
    Since the 1930’s, several attempts have been made to develop a general theory of technical systems or objects and their evolution: in France, Jacques Lafitte, André Leroi-Gourhan, Bertrand Gille, Yves Deforge, and Gilbert Simondon are the main representatives of this trend. In this paper, we focus on the work of Simondon: his analysis of technical progress is based on the hypothesis that technology has its own laws and that customer demand has no paramount influence upon the evolution of technical (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  27
    Gilbert Simondon’s genetic “mecanology”and the understanding of laws of technical evolution.Vincent Bontems - 2009 - Techné: Research in Philosophy and Technology 13 (1):1-12.
    Since the 1930’s, several attempts have been made to develop a general theory of technical systems or objects and their evolution: in France, Jacques Lafitte, André Leroi-Gourhan, Bertrand Gille, Yves Deforge, and Gilbert Simondon are the main representatives of this trend. In this paper, we focus on the work of Simondon: his analysis of technical progress is based on the hypothesis that technology has its own laws and that customer demand has no paramount influence upon the evolution of technical (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  62
    The Overlap Feature of the Genetic Equidistance Result—A Fundamental Biological Phenomenon Overlooked for Nearly Half of a Century.Shi Huang - 2010 - Biological Theory 5 (1):40-52.
    The genetic equidistance result shows that different species are approximately equidistant to a simpler outgroup in protein sequence similarity, as first reported by Margoliash in 1963. This result, together with those of Zuckerkandl and Pauling in 1962 inspired the molecular clock and in turn the neutral theory of evolution. Here it is shown that the clock/neutral theory had from the beginning overlooked another characteristic of the equidistance result, the overlap feature, which shows a large number of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  16
    Why the Anti-reductionist Consensus Won’t Survive the Case of Classical Mendelian Genetics.C. Kenneth Waters - 1990 - PSA Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1990 (1):125-139.
    Philosophers now treat the relationship between Classical Mendelian Genetics and molecular biology as a paradigm of nonreduction and this example is playing an increasingly prominent role in debates about the reducibility of theories ranging from macrosocial science to folk psychology. Patricia Churchland (1986), for example, draws an analogy between the alleged elimination of the “causal mainstay” of classical genetics and her view that today’s psychological theory will be eliminated by neuroscience. Patricia Kitcher takes an autonomous rather than eliminativist view (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  17.  9
    Taxonomy and Theory.A. Aaron Snyder - 1982 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1982:512 - 521.
    Biological evolution allegedly requires a genealogical conception of species (i.e., that species are descent-based "historical entities" rather than similarity-based "natural kinds"). After considering David Hull's arguments for this view, this paper opts instead for individuating species primarily via genetic similarities, but in a way which avoids charges of "Essentialism". It is suggested that a genealogical conception of species actually derives from a biological version of Behaviorism plus an interrelated pair of confusions regarding evolution and identity. Current taxonomic method (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  66
    Genetic similarity, human altruism, and group selection.J. Philippe Rushton - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):503-518.
  19.  22
    From the Cellular Standpoint: is DNA Sequence Genetic ‘Information’?Steven S. D. C. Rubin - 2017 - Biosemiotics 10 (2):247-264.
    Constructivist biosemiotics foundations imply the first-person basis of cognition. CBF are developed by the biology of cognition, relational biology, enactive approach, ecology of mind, second order cybernetics, genetic epistemology, gestalt, ecological perception and affordances, and active inference by minimization of free energy. CBF reject the idea of an objective independent reality to be represented by information processing in order to be the fittest. CBF assumes that perception is the behavioral configuration of an object and objects are tokens for eigen-behaviors. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  20.  81
    Evolutionary theory and group selection: The question of warfare.Doyne Dawson - 1999 - History and Theory 38 (4):79–100.
    Evolutionary anthropology has focused on the origins of war, or rather ethnocentricity, because it epitomizes the problem of group selection, and because war may itself have been the main agent of group selection. The neo-Darwinian synthesis in biology has explained how ethnocentricity might evolve by group selection, and the distinction between evoked culture and adopted culture, suggested by the emerging synthesis in evolutionary psychology, has explained how it might be transmitted. Ethnocentric mechanisms could have evolved by genetic selection in (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  21.  53
    Niche construction theory as an explanatory framework for human phenomena.Efraim Wallach - 2016 - Synthese 193 (8).
    Niche Construction Theory has been gaining acceptance as an explanatory framework for processes in biological and human evolution. Human cultural niche construction, in particular, is suggested as a basis for understanding many phenomena that involve human genetic and cultural evolution. Herein I assess the ability of the cultural niche construction framework to meet this explanatory role by looking into several NCT-inspired accounts that have been offered for two important episodes of human evolution, and by examining the contribution of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  22.  28
    A theory on the ancestry of angiosperms.Hui -Lin Li - 1960 - Acta Biotheoretica 13 (4):185-202.
    By inferences from fossil records and circumstantial evidences, it is now generally postulated that angiosperms have a much longer history than hitherto believed and that they have already existed probably in Jurassic time. Studies in vascular tissues and reproductive, structures have negated the possibility of originating angiosperms from various gymnosperm groups. Chronologically, this derivation will be also an impossibility.From a consideration of various aspects in the life history of angiosperms, a hypothesis is here presented postulating that protangiosperms originated in an (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  8
    Process Narratives, Grey Boxes, and Discourse Frameworks: Cognition, Interaction, and Constraint in Understanding Genetics and Medicine.Barry Saferstein - 2007 - European Journal of Social Theory 10 (3):424-447.
    The article presents a model of understanding that takes into account interaction, cultural knowledge, and the constraints of organizations and institutions. It analyzes discourse and cognition in high school biology classes and clinical consultations involving discussions of genetics. The analytical lenses of constraint satisfaction, coherence-based reasoning, and collective cognition reveal multilayered social, cultural, and interactional components of authority and agency that influence understanding. The analysis reveals similarities across settings in discourse structure and the ways that participants relate to local constraints. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  20
    Visualizing genetic similarity at the symptom level: The example of learning disabilities.Oliver Sp Davis & Robert Plomin - 2010 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2-3):155-157.
    Psychological traits and disorders are often interrelated through shared genetic influences. A combination of maximum-likelihood structural equation modelling and multidimensional scaling enables us to open a window onto the genetic architecture at the symptom level, rather than at the level of latent genetic factors. We illustrate this approach using a study of cognitive abilities involving over 5,000 pairs of twins.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  11
    Promoting diagnostic equity: specifying genetic similarity rather than race or ethnicity.Katherine Witte Saylor & Daphne Oluwaseun Martschenko - 2023 - Journal of Medical Ethics 49 (12):820-821.
    In their article on the limited duty to reinterpret genetic variants, Watts and Newson argue that clinical labs are not morally obligated to conduct routine reinterpretation despite its potential clinical and personal value.1 We endorse the authors’ argument for a circumscribed duty to reclassify genomic variants in certain cases, including to promote diagnostic equity for racial and ethnic minority populations that have been historically excluded from and exploited by genomic research and medicine. However, given the history and resilience of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  26.  8
    Testing genetic similarity: Out of control.John Hartung - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):529-530.
  27. Detecting genetic similarity without detecting genetic similarity.Dennis Krebs - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):533-534.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  22
    Genetic similarity, human altruism and group selection: A study of the open peer commentaries.Klaus Jaffe - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):525-526.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  3
    Genetic similarity between friends and lovers: Is an evolutionary view warranted?Harold Gouzoules - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):526-527.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  21
    Can there be a theory of development?: Allessandro Minelli and Thomas Pradeu : Towards a theory of development. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2014, 304pp, £37.50 PB, £75.00 HB.Kostas Kampourakis - 2015 - Metascience 24 (2):199-203.
    When I was an undergraduate student in biology, about twenty years ago, developmental biology was relatively absent in my curriculum. There were some elements of developmental biology in the zoology and botany courses, but one had to take two elective courses, Embryology and Molecular Biology of Development, in order to learn more. Fortunately, curricula have changed nowadays and for good reasons. The study of developmental processes is crucial for our understanding of life, perhaps more than ever. For example, it is (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31. Numerical testing of evolution theories.Nils Aall Barricelli - 1962 - Acta Biotheoretica 16 (1):69-98.
    An interpretive system for the IBM 704 computer permitting interpretation of the genetic pattern of a numeric symbioorganism as a game strategy has been developed. Selection for best performance in a simple game has been applied in a preliminary experiment. An objective method to measure the quality of a game played is described. The results presented in the article show a small but significant improvement of game quality during a period of 2300 generations.The general characteristics of the phenomena observed (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32. Simulating Grice: Emergent Pragmatics in Spatialized Game Theory.Patrick Grim - 2011 - In Anton Benz, Christian Ebert & Robert van Rooij (eds.), Language, Games, and Evolution. Springer-Verlag.
    How do conventions of communication emerge? How do sounds or gestures take on a semantic meaning, and how do pragmatic conventions emerge regarding the passing of adequate, reliable, and relevant information? My colleagues and I have attempted in earlier work to extend spatialized game theory to questions of semantics. Agent-based simulations indicate that simple signaling systems emerge fairly naturally on the basis of individual information maximization in environments of wandering food sources and predators. Simple signaling emerges by means of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  33.  31
    Qu'est-ce qu'une grande theorie biologique?Michel Delsol & Janine Flatin - 1991 - Acta Biotheoretica 39 (3-4):363-373.
    La parution récente en français du livre de M. Denton : “Evolution. Une théorie en crise” , qui traite des theories explicatives actuelles de l'évolution, nous amine à rappeler les caracteres généraux des grandes theories biologiques et à présenter une critique sommaire du livre en question.La science West pas une simple accumulation de connaissances. Le scientifique ne doit pas se contenter de decrire et de mesurer des faits. Son but eat d'essayer de les relier et de construire des théories qui (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  11
    Recognising kin = Recognising genetic similarity.P. G. Hepper - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):530-530.
  35.  23
    Age similarity is genetic similarity.J. Philippe Rushton - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):108-108.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  26
    Assortative Pairing and Life History Strategy.Aurelio José Figueredo & Pedro S. A. Wolf - 2009 - Human Nature 20 (3):317-330.
    A secondary analysis was performed on preliminary data from an ongoing cross-cultural study on assortative pairing. Independently sampled pairs of opposite-sex romantic partners and of same-sex friends rated themselves and each other on Life History (LH) strategy and mate value. Data were collected in local bars, clubs, coffeehouses, and other public places from three different cultures: Tucson, Arizona; Hermosillo, Sonora; and San José, Costa Rica. The present analysis found that slow LH individuals assortatively pair with both sexual and social partners (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  37. Population genetics, economic theory, and eugenics in R.A. Fisher.Jean Gayon - 2014 - In R. Paul Thompson & Denis Walsh (eds.), Evolutionary biology: conceptual, ethical, and religious issues. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  1
    Introduction: Genet and Theory.Mairéad Hanrahan - 2004 - Paragraph 27 (2):1-6.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  29
    Measure and representation of the genetic similarity between populations by the percentage of isoactive genes.Alicia Sánchez-Mazas, Laurent Excoffier & André Langaney - 1986 - Theoria 2 (1):143-154.
    A similarity index allowing comparisons of human populations has been defined as the common “Percentage of Isoactive Genes” or PIG, which can be calculated from any gene frequency distribution characterizing two populations. The complement to one of this value has been proved to be a distance, a measure which can be used in most techniques of cluster analysis as well as in usual representations of multivariated data (dendrograms, etc...). Furthermore, the formula can be generalized to a set of populations. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  10
    A methodological critique of the evidence for genetic similarity detection.Judith L. Anderson - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):518-519.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  9
    The role of genes in genetic similarity detection.Ian Vine - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):545-546.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  20
    Why birds of a feather flock together: Genetic similarity?David C. Rowe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):540-541.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  6
    From similarity to uniqueness: Method and theory in comparative psychology.Ingar Brinck - 2008 - In Louise Röska-Hardy & Eva M. Neumann-Held (eds.), Learning from Animals? Examining the Nature of Human Uniqueness. London: Psychology Press.
    Comparative psychology is a strongly interdisciplinary field that shares many of its experimental methods and observational techniques with ethology and developmental psychology. The great variety of theories that comparative psychology evokes to explain behavior generates a wide array of exciting and potentially fruitful accounts, but is also problematic. It increases the risk of error in the forms of inconsistent background assumptions, conceptual misunderstandings, unfalsifiable hypotheses and incoherent explanations, which in spite of perhaps being minor by themselves will impede scientific progress (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  8
    Similarity: a paradigm for culture theory.Anil Bhatti & Dorothee Kimmich (eds.) - 2018 - New Delhi, India: Tulika Books.
    Papers presented in three conferences, supported by the Fritz Thyssen Foundation, the Excellence Cluster 16: 'Cultural Foundation of Integration' at the University of Konstanz, and the Institute of German Studies and the Forum Scientairum at the Univesity of Tubingen.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  1
    Genetic theory of reality.James Mark Baldwin - 1915 - and London,: G. P. Putnam's sons.
    James Mark Baldwin left a legacy that has yet to be fully examined, one with profound implications for science and the humanities. In some sense it paralleled that of his friend Charles Sanders Peirce, whose semiotics became understood only a century later. Baldwin was trying to make sense of complex biological and social processes that only now have come into the limelight as biological sciences have re-emerged in psychology. Baldwin's focus on development, based on the observation of his own children (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46. Theory change in science: strategies from Mendelian genetics.Lindley Darden - 1991 - New York: Oxford University Press.
    This innovative book focuses on the development of the gene theory as a case study in scientific creativity.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   146 citations  
  47. Genetics and justice, non-ideal theory and the role of patents : the case of CRISPR-Cas9.Oliver Feeney - 2022 - In Santa Slokenberga, Timo Minssen & Ana Nordberg (eds.), Governing, protecting, and regulating the future of genome editing: the significance of ELSPI perspectives. Boston: Brill/Nijhoff.
  48.  33
    Lindsay Craig—The So-Called Extended Synthesis and Population Genetics (Biological Theory 5: 117–123, 2010): Extended Synthesis: Theory Expansion or Alternative? [REVIEW]Gerd B. Müller & Massimo Pigliucci - 2010 - Biological Theory 5 (3):275-276.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  49. Genetic information: A metaphor in search of a theory.Paul Edmund Griffiths - 2001 - Philosophy of Science 68 (3):394-412.
    John Maynard Smith has defended against philosophical criticism the view that developmental biology is the study of the expression of information encoded in the genes by natural selection. However, like other naturalistic concepts of information, this ‘teleosemantic’ information applies to many non-genetic factors in development. Maynard Smith also fails to show that developmental biology is concerned with teleosemantic information. Some other ways to support Maynard Smith’s conclusion are considered. It is argued that on any definition of information the view (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   127 citations  
  50.  30
    Classical genetics and the theory-net of genetics.Pablo Lorenzano - 2000 - In Joseph D. Sneed, Wolfgang Balzer & C.-Ulises Moulines (eds.), Structuralist Knowledge Representation: Paradigmatic Examples. Rodopi. pp. 75-251.
    This article presents a reconstruction of the so-called classical, formal or Mendelian genetics, which is intended to be more complete and adequate than existing reconstructions. This reconstruction has been carried out with the instruments, duly modified and extended with respect to the case under consideration, of the structuralist conception of theories. The so-called Mendel’s Laws, as well as linkage genetics and gene mapping are formulated in a precise manner while the global structure of genetics is represented as a theory-net. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
1 — 50 / 981