9 found
Order:
  1.  19
    Short-term memory for spatial location in goal-directed locomotion.Digby Elliott, Ruth Jones & Susan Gray - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (2):158-160.
  2.  25
    Visual-spatial movement goals.Digby Elliott & Brian K. V. Maraj - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):207-207.
  3.  11
    Intra- and interhemispheric integration of tactual and visual spatial information.Ruth Jones & Digby Elliott - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (3):229-231.
  4.  20
    Temporal judgements of internal and external events in persons with and without autism.Cheryl M. Glazebrook, Digby Elliott & James Lyons - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):203-209.
    When participants make judgments about the onset of self-initiated movements they typically report the movement occurred earlier than it had [Obhi, S. S., & Haggard, P. . Free will and free won’t. American Scientific, 92, 358–365.]. One interpretation is that feed-forward processes lead to awareness of the movement prior to execution. Because individuals with autism experience reduced preparatory activity prior to a voluntary movement, the present study sought to determine whether these anticipatory biases are exhibited by persons with autism. Participants (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  28
    Visual control of target-directed movements.Romeo Chua & Digby Elliott - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (2):304-306.
    Visual feedback regulation during movement is not fully captured in Plamondon's kinematic theory. However, numerous studies indicate that visual response-produced feedback is a powerful determinant of performance and kinematic characteristics of target-directed movement.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  45
    A fast ventral stream or early dorsal-ventral interactions?Digby Elliott, Luc Tremblay & Timothy N. Welsh - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (1):105-105.
    Several lines of evidence indicate that rapid target-aiming movements, involving both the eyes and hand, can be biased by the visual context in which the movements are performed. Some of these contextual influences carry-over from trial to trial. This research indicates that dissociation between the dorsal and ventral systems based on speed, conscious awareness, and frame of reference is far from clear.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  14
    Human handedness reconsidered.Digby Elliott - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):341-342.
  8.  17
    Visual context can influence on-line control.Digby Elliott & Daniel V. Meegan - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (1):33-34.
    Several lines of evidence indicate that the on-line control of rapid target-aiming movements can be influenced by the visual context in which the movements are performed. Although this may result in movement error when an illusory context is introduced, there are many situations in which the control system must know about context in order to get the limb to the target rapidly and safely.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  5
    Atypical cerebral dominance in Down’s syndrome.Daniel J. Weeks & Digby Elliott - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (1):23-25.