70 found
Order:
Disambiguations
David Gooding [48]David C. Gooding [11]Diane C. Gooding [6]D. C. Gooding [5]
D. Gooding [1]
  1.  29
    The Uses of Experiment: Studies in the Natural Sciences.David Gooding, Trevor Pinch & Simon Schaffer - 1989 - Cambridge University Press. Edited by David Gooding, Trevor Pinch & Simon Schaffer.
    Contributors; Preface; Introduction; Part I. Instruments in Experiments: 1. Scientific instruments: models of brass and aids to discovery; 2. Glass works: Newton’s prisms and the uses of experiment; 3. A viol of water or a wedge of glass; Part II. Experiment and Argument: 4. Galileo’s experimental discourse; 5. Fresnel, Poisson and the white spot: the role of successful predictions in the acceptance of scientific theories; 6. The rhetoric of experiment; Part III. Representing and Realising: 7. ’Magnetic curves’ and the magnetic (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  2. The Uses of Experiment.David Gooding, Trevor Pinch & Simon Schaffer - 1992 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 43 (1):99-109.
  3. Experiment Right or Wrong.Allan Franklin & David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45 (1):341-352.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  4. What is Experimental about Thought Experiments?David C. Gooding - 1992 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1992:280 - 290.
    I argue that thought experiments are a form of experimental reasoning similar to real experiments. They require the same ability to participate by following a narrative as real experiments do. Participation depends in turn on using what we already know to visualize, manipulate and understand what is unfamiliar or problematic. I defend the claim that visualization requires embodiment by an example which shows how tacit understanding of the properties of represented objects and relations enables us to work out how such (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  5.  19
    Scientific and Technological Thinking.M. Gorman, R. Tweney, D. Gooding & A. Kincannon (eds.) - 2005 - Erlbaum.
    This book describes empirically ways to analyze and then to effectually utilize cognitive processes to advance discovery and invention in the sciences. It also explains how to teach these principles to students.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  6.  57
    How do Scientists Reach Agreement about Novel Observations?David Gooding - 1986 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 17 (2):205.
    I outline a pragmatic view of scientists' use of observation which draws attention to non-discursive, instrumental and social contexts of observation, in order to explain scientists' agreement about the appearance and significance of new phenomena. I argue that: observation is embedded in a network of activities, techniques, and interests; that experimentalists make construals of new phenomena which enable them communicate exploratory techniques and their outcomes, and that empirical enquiry consists of communicative, exploratory and predictive strategies whose interdependence ensures that, notwithstanding (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  7.  12
    Empiricism in Practice: Teleology, Economy, and Observation in Faraday's Physics.David Gooding - 1982 - Isis 73:46-67.
  8.  15
    Empiricism in Practice: Teleology, Economy, and Observation in Faraday's Physics.David Gooding - 1982 - Isis 73 (1):46-67.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  9.  77
    Visualizing Scientific Inference.David C. Gooding - 2010 - Topics in Cognitive Science 2 (1):15-35.
    The sciences use a wide range of visual devices, practices, and imaging technologies. This diversity points to an important repertoire of visual methods that scientists use to adapt representations to meet the varied demands that their work places on cognitive processes. This paper identifies key features of the use of visualization in a range of scientific domains and considers the implications of this repertoire for understanding scientists as cognitive agents.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  10. Simulation Methods for an Abductive System in Science.D. C. Gooding & T. R. Addis - 2008 - Foundations of Science 13 (1):37-52.
    Syntactic and structural models specify relationships between their constituents but cannot show what outcomes their interaction would produce over time in the world. Simulation consists in iterating the states of a model, so as to produce behaviour over a period of simulated time. Iteration enables us to trace the implications and outcomes of inference rules and other assumptions implemented in the models that make up a theory. We apply this method to experiments which we treat as models of the particular (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  11.  18
    Cognition, Construction and Culture: Visual Theories in the Sciences.David Gooding - 2004 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 4 (3-4):551-593.
    This paper presents a study of the generation, manipulation and use of visual representations in different episodes of scientific discovery. The study identifies a common set of transformations of visual representations underlying the distinctive methods and imagery of different scientific fields. The existence of common features behind the diversity of visual representations suggests a common dynamical structure for visual thinking, showing how visual representations facilitate cognitive processes such as pattern-matching and visual inference through the use of tools, technologies and other (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  12.  91
    Simulation Methods for an Abductive System in Science.Tom Addis, Jan Townsend Addis, Dave Billinge, David Gooding & Bart-Floris Visscher - 2008 - Foundations of Science 13 (1):37-52.
    We argue that abduction does not work in isolation from other inference mechanisms and illustrate this through an inference scheme designed to evaluate multiple hypotheses. We use game theory to relate the abductive system to actions that produce new information. To enable evaluation of the implications of this approach we have implemented the procedures used to calculate the impact of new information in a computer model. Experiments with this model display a number of features of collective belief-revision leading to consensus-formation, (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  13. The Procedural Turn; or, Why Do Thought Experiments Work?David Gooding - 1992 - In R. Giere & H. Feigl (eds.), Cognitive Models of Science. University of Minnesota Press. pp. 45-76.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  14.  7
    Mapping Experiment as a Learning Process: How the First Electromagnetic Motor Was Invented.David Gooding - 1990 - Science, Technology and Human Values 15 (2):165-201.
    Narrative accounts misrepresent discovery by reconstructing worlds ordered by success rather than the world as explored. Such worlds rarely contain the personal knowledge that informed actual exploration and experiment. This article describes an attempt to recover situated learning in a material environment, tracing the discovery of the first electromagnetic motor by Michael Faraday in September 1821 to show how he modeled new experience and invented procedures to communicate that novelty. The author introduces a notation to map experiment as an active (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  15.  71
    From phenomenology to field theory: Faraday's visual reasoning.David C. Gooding - 2006 - Perspectives on Science 14 (1):40-65.
    : Faraday is often described as an experimentalist, but his work is a dialectical interplay of concrete objects, visual images, abstract, theoretically-informed visual models and metaphysical precepts. From phenomena described in terms of patterns formed by lines of force he created a general explanation of space-filling systems of force which obey both empirical laws and principles of conservation and economy. I argue that Faraday's articulation of situated experience via visual models into a theory capable of verbal expression owed much to (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  16. Simulation Methods for an Abductive System in Science.T. R. Addis & D. C. Gooding - 2008 - Foundations of Science 13 (1):37-52.
    We argue that abduction does not work in isolation from other inference mechanisms and illustrate this through an inference scheme designed to evaluate multiple hypotheses. We use game theory to relate the abductive system to actions that produce new information. To enable evaluation of the implications of this approach we have implemented the procedures used to calculate the impact of new information in a computer model. Experiments with this model display a number of features of collective belief-revision leading to consensus-formation, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  17. The Future of Cognitive Studies of Science and Technology.Michael E. Gorman, Ryan D. Tweney, David C. Gooding & Alexandra P. Kincannon - 2005 - In M. Gorman, R. Tweney, D. Gooding & A. Kincannon (eds.), Scientific and Technological Thinking. Erlbaum.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  18.  27
    Creative Rationality: towards an Abductive Model of Scientific Change.David Gooding - 1996 - Philosophica 58 (2).
  19. How Scientists Reach Agreement about New Observations.David Gooding - 1986 - PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1986:236-244.
    I outline a pragmatic view of scientists' use of observation which draws attention to non-discursive, instrumental and social contexts of observation, in order to explain scientists' agreement about the appearance and significance of new phenomena. I argue that: observation is embedded in a network of activities, techniques, and interests; that experimentalists make construals of new phenomena which enable them communicate exploratory techniques and their outcomes, and that empirical enquiry consists of communicative, exploratory and predictive strategies whose interdependence ensures that, notwithstanding (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  20.  17
    Metaphysics versus measurement: The conversion and conservation of force in Faraday's physics.David Gooding - 1980 - Annals of Science 37 (1):1-29.
    SummaryFaraday's concept of force is described by six assumptions. These specify a concept that is quite distinct from ‘mechanical’ conceptions of his contemporaries and interpreters. Analysis of the role of these assumptions clarifies Faraday's weighting of experimental evidence and shows how closely-linked Faraday's chemistry and physics were to his theology. It is argued that Faraday was unable to secularize his concept of force by breaking the ties between his physics and his theology of nature. Examination of his basic assumptions also (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  21.  16
    Hedonic Capacity in the Broader Autism Phenotype: Should Social Anhedonia Be Considered a Characteristic Feature?Derek M. Novacek, Diane C. Gooding & Madeline J. Pflum - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  22.  70
    Visual cognition: Where cognition and culture meet.David C. Gooding - 2006 - Philosophy of Science 73 (5):688-698.
    Case studies of diverse scientific fields show how scientists use a range of resources to generate new interpretative models and to establish their plausibility as explanations of a domain. They accomplish this by manipulating imagistic representations in particular ways. I show that scientists in different domains use the same basic transformations. Common features of these transformations indicate that general cognitive strategies of interpretation, simplification, elaboration, and argumentation are at work. Social and historical studies of science emphasize the diversity of local (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  23. Seeing the forest for the trees: Visualization, cognition, and scientific inference.David C. Gooding - 2005 - In M. Gorman, R. Tweney, D. Gooding & A. Kincannon (eds.), Scientific and Technological Thinking. Erlbaum. pp. 2005--173.
  24.  31
    Conceptual and Experimental Bases of Faraday's Denial of Electrostatic Action at a Distance.David Gooding - 1978 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 9 (2):117.
  25.  4
    The Predictive Value of the NEO-FFI Items: Parsing the Nature of Social Anhedonia Using the Revised Social Anhedonia Scale and the ACIPS.Diane C. Gooding, Emily R. Padrutt & Madeline J. Pflum - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26.  32
    Faraday, Thomson, and the Concept of the Magnetic Field.David Gooding - 1980 - British Journal for the History of Science 13 (2):91-120.
    In June 1849 William Thomson wrote to Michael Faraday suggesting that the concept of a uniform magnetic field could be used to predict the motions of small magnetic and diamagnetic bodies. In his letter Thomson showed how Faraday's lines of magnetic force could represent the effect of the ‘conducting power’ for magnetic force of matter in the region of magnets. This was Thomson's extension to magnetism of an analogy between the mathematical descriptions of the distribution of static electricity and of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  27.  11
    Experiment.David C. Gooding - 2017 - In W. H. Newton‐Smith (ed.), A Companion to the Philosophy of Science. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. pp. 117–126.
    There have been many images of experiment. The contemplative narratives of Aristotle served to illustrate hypotheses and arguments. There was no expectation that they be performed. Even in Galileo's dialogues, the distinction between real experiments and imaginary ones is not sharp (see galileo). During the seventeenth century, performance and public description became essential to the probative power of experiment. These made its methods and procedures transparent, allowing any reader of the narrative to be a virtual witness of an active demonstration (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  6
    Polygenic risk scores cannot make their mark on psychiatry without considering epigenetics.Diane C. Gooding & Anthony P. Auger - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e216.
    We generally agree with Burt's thesis. However, we note that the author did not discuss epigenetics, the study of how the environment can alter gene structure and function. Given epigenetic mechanisms, the utility of polygenic risk scores (PRS) is limited in studies of development and mental illness. Finally, in this commentary we expand upon the risks of reliance upon PRSs.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  10
    Esperimento e realismo scientifico: saggio su David Gooding.Marco Buzzoni & David Gooding - 2001 - Napoli: Loffredo. Edited by David Gooding.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  3
    Cockroft and the AtomGuy Hartcup T. E. Allibone.David Gooding - 1985 - Isis 76 (4):610-611.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  49
    Cognitive coordination deficits: A necessary but not sufficient factor in the development of schizophrenia.Diane C. Gooding & Jacqueline G. Braun - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (1):89-90.
    The Phillips & Silverstein model of NMDA-mediated coordination deficits provides a useful heuristic for the study of schizophrenic cognition. However, the model does not specifically account for the development of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. The P&S model is compared to Meehl's seminal model of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia, as well as the model of schizophrenic cognitive dysfunction posited by McCarley and colleagues.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  12
    Energy: Historical Development of the ConceptR. Bruce Lindsay.David C. Gooding - 1977 - Isis 68 (3):464-465.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33. Faraday and the Powers of Matter the Role of Principles, Hypotheses, and the Interpretation of Experiment in the Development of Faraday's Field Theory, as Presented in His Experimental Researches in Electricity, 1830-1855.David Gooding - 1975
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  2
    Faraday to Einstein: Constructing Meaning in Scientific TheoriesNancy J. Nersessian.David Gooding - 1986 - Isis 77 (3):521-523.
  35.  4
    How Scientists reach Agreement about new Observations.David Gooding - 1986 - PSA Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1986 (1):236-244.
    Epistemology has been socialised. Cognitive and social interactions between observers are now as important as their interventions into the course of nature. This contrasts with traditional views of how we get natural facts into our discourse about nature. These assumed the efficacy of individual observers’ causal interactions with the natural world, making their interactions with other observers irrelevant. Kant’s conclusion that empirical access depends also upon our concepts did not challenge the individualistic character of the epistemology of science. Observers’ agreement (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36. Review of U nderstanding Philosophy of Science.David Gooding - 2003 - Philosophical Books 44 (3):291-291.
  37.  21
    Philosophy and science: The Black box again.D. C. Gooding - 1974 - Metaphilosophy 5 (1):69–69.
  38.  2
    Re-presenting Faraday.David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the History of Science 27 (3):361-364.
  39.  6
    Scientific Practice: Theories and Stories of Doing Physics. Jed Z. Buchwald.David Gooding - 1997 - Isis 88 (1):121-122.
  40.  39
    Theory and observation: The experimental nexus.David Gooding - 1990 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 4 (2):131 – 148.
    Abstract Philosophical discussions of experiment usually focus exclusively on testing predictions. In this paper I compare G. Morpurgo's experimental test of the Gell?Mann/ Zweig quark hypothesis with two neglected uses of experiment: constructing representations of new phenomena and inventing the instruments that produce such phenomena. These roles are illustrated by J. B. Biot's 1821 observations of electromagnetism and by Michael Faraday's invention of the first electromagnetic motor, also in 1821. The comparison identifies similarities between observation and experiment, showing how both (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  5
    Philosophy and Science: The Black Box Again.D. C. Gooding - 1974 - Metaphilosophy 5 (1):69-69.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42.  6
    The Concept of Matter in Modern PhilosophyErnan McMullin.David C. Gooding - 1980 - Isis 71 (3):486-486.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  30
    The role of executive control in saccade generation.Diane C. Gooding - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):686-687.
    The Findlay & Walker model of saccade generation does not appear to account fully for saccadic performance deficits observed in schizophrenia patients. It would be enhanced by inclusion of a frontally mediated, central executive function system. A review of schizophrenia patients' antisaccade task deficits provides an example of the role of higher cortical functioning in saccade generation.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  20
    The Rediscovery of the Mind. John R. Searle.David C. Gooding - 1994 - Isis 85 (2):362-363.
  45.  34
    What can particle physicists count on?David Gooding, William J. McKinney, Harry M. Marks, Jeff Hughes & Alan Chalmers - 1999 - Metascience 8 (3):356-392.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  11
    Validation of the Korean Version of the Anticipatory and Consummatory Interpersonal Pleasure Scale in Non-help-seeking Individuals.Eunhye Kim, Diane C. Gooding & Tae Young Lee - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    The Anticipatory and Consummatory Interpersonal Pleasure Scale is a psychometric instrument that has been used to indirectly measure social anhedonia in many cross-cultural contexts, such as in Western, European, Eastern, and Israeli samples. However, little is known about the psychometric properties of the ACIPS in Korean samples. The primary goal of this study was to validate the Korean version of the ACIPS among non-help-seeking individuals. The sample consisted of 307 adult individuals who had no current or prior psychiatric history. Participants (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. Imaginary science. [REVIEW]David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45 (4):1029-1045.
  48.  10
    Review: Imaginary Science. [REVIEW]David Gooding - 1994 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45 (4):1029 - 1045.
  49.  27
    How to Be a Good Empiricist. [REVIEW]David Gooding - 1989 - British Journal for the History of Science 22 (4):419-427.
  50.  21
    Cockroft and the Atom by Guy Hartcup; T. E. Allibone. [REVIEW]David Gooding - 1985 - Isis 76:610-611.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 70