13 found
Order:
Disambiguations
Cynthia Fisher [12]Cynthia L. Fisher [1]
  1. Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience.Kyle E. Chambers, Kristine H. Onishi & Cynthia Fisher - 2003 - Cognition 87 (2):B69-B77.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  2.  57
    2.5-Year-olds use cross-situational consistency to learn verbs under referential uncertainty.Rose M. Scott & Cynthia Fisher - 2012 - Cognition 122 (2):163-180.
  3. The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: a reply to Tomasello.Cynthia Fisher - 2002 - Cognition 82 (3):259-278.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  4.  41
    The Developmental Origins of Syntactic Bootstrapping.Cynthia Fisher, Kyong-sun Jin & Rose M. Scott - 2020 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):48-77.
    Fisher, Jin, and Scott push a central assumption of syntactic bootstrapping: that learners have a universal bias to map each noun in a sentence onto a participant role (i.e., argument of the verb). They propose two enrichments: First, that children use both semantic and syntactic information in representing nouns that accompany a verb; second, that children expect continuity across a discourse. They provide evidence for both learning mechanisms among young children, further spelling out the precise mechanisms underlying syntactic bootstrapping.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  5.  27
    Where are the cookies? Two- and three-year-olds use number-marked verbs to anticipate upcoming nouns.Cynthia Lukyanenko & Cynthia Fisher - 2016 - Cognition 146 (C):349-370.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  6.  28
    Mistakes weren’t made: Three-year-olds’ comprehension of novel-verb passives provides evidence for early abstract syntax.Katherine Messenger & Cynthia Fisher - 2018 - Cognition 178 (C):118-132.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  7.  50
    Predicted errors in children’s early sentence comprehension.Yael Gertner & Cynthia Fisher - 2012 - Cognition 124 (1):85-94.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  8. Language acquisition.Cynthia Fisher & Lila R. Gleitman - 2002 - In J. Wixted & H. Pashler, Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  9.  54
    Can infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action?Hyun-joo Song, Renée Baillargeon & Cynthia Fisher - 2005 - Cognition 98 (2):B45-B55.
  10.  46
    Speech errors reflect the phonotactic constraints in recently spoken syllables, but not in recently heard syllables.Jill A. Warker, Ye Xu, Gary S. Dell & Cynthia Fisher - 2009 - Cognition 112 (1):81-96.
  11.  31
    6 Universal aspects of word learning.Lila Gleitman & Cynthia Fisher - 2005 - In James McGilvray, The Cambridge Companion to Chomsky. Cambridge University Press. pp. 123.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  12. Not all subjects are agents : transitivity and meaning in early language comprehension.Rose M. Scott, Yael Gertner & Cynthia Fisher - 2018 - In Kristen Syrett & Sudha Arunachalam, Semantics in language acquisition. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  13.  19
    What’s New to You? Preschoolers’ Partner-Specific Online Processing of Disfluency.Si On Yoon, Kyong-sun Jin, Sarah Brown-Schmidt & Cynthia L. Fisher - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Speech disfluencies can signal that a speaker is about to refer to something difficult to name. In two experiments, we found evidence that 4-year-olds, like adults, flexibly interpret a particular partner’s disfluency based on their estimate of that partner’s knowledge, derived from the preceding conversation. In entrainment trials, children established partner-specific shared knowledge of names for tangram pictures with one or two adult interlocutors. In each test trial, an adult named one of two visible tangrams either fluently or disfluently while (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark