Results for ' simple motor learning'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  15
    Variation of postfeedback interval in simple motor learning.Dorothe R. Weinberg, Donald E. Guy & Ronald W. Tupper - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (1):98.
  2. Christian Mannes.Learning Sensory-Motor Coordination Experimentation - 1990 - In G. Dorffner (ed.), Konnektionismus in Artificial Intelligence Und Kognitionsforschung. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. pp. 95.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  21
    Learning of a simple grapho-motor task by young children and adults: similar acquisition but age-dependent retention.Mona S. Julius & Esther Adi-Japha - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  4.  14
    The common inverse-dynamics motor-command coordinates for complex and simple spikes.M. Kawato - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):462-464.
    Recent advanced statistical analysis of complex spikes has revealed that their instantaneous firing rate within a time bin of a few milliseconds carries information if many trials are averaged, as happens in motor learning. The firing rate encodes sensory error signals in the inverse-dynamics motor-command coordinates, and these are exactly the same coordinates as for simple spikes. This strongly supports the most critical assumption of the feedback-error-learning model and argues against several hypotheses about the functions (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  9
    Lost in Translation: Simple Steps in Experimental Design of Neurorehabilitation-Based Research Interventions to Promote Motor Recovery Post-Stroke.Natalia Sánchez & Carolee J. Winstein - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Stroke continues to be a leading cause of disability. Basic neurorehabilitation research is necessary to inform the neuropathophysiology of impaired motor control, and to develop targeted interventions with potential to remediate disability post-stroke. Despite knowledge gained from basic research studies, the effectiveness of research-based interventions for reducing motor impairment has been no greater than standard of practice interventions. In this perspective, we offer suggestions for overcoming translational barriers integral to experimental design, to augment traditional protocols, and re-route the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  4
    Effects of the Proprioceptive Neuromuscular Facilitation Contraction Sequence on Motor Skill Learning-Related Increases in the Maximal Rate of Wrist Flexion Torque Development.Lara A. Green, Jessica McGuire & David A. Gabriel - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Background: The proprioceptive neuromuscular facilitation reciprocal contraction pattern has the potential to increase the maximum rate of torque development. However, it is a more complex resistive exercise task and may interfere with improvements in the maximum rate of torque development due to motor skill learning, as observed for unidirectional contractions. The purpose of this study was to examine the cost-benefit of using the PNF exercise technique to increase the maximum rate of torque development.Methods: Twenty-six participants completed isometric maximal (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  30
    Acquisition and long-term retention of a simple serial perceptual-motor skill.Eva Neumann & R. B. Ammons - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (3):159.
  8.  15
    Predictive Movements and Human Reinforcement Learning of Sequential Action.Roy Kleijn, George Kachergis & Bernhard Hommel - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S3):783-808.
    Sequential action makes up the bulk of human daily activity, and yet much remains unknown about how people learn such actions. In one motor learning paradigm, the serial reaction time (SRT) task, people are taught a consistent sequence of button presses by cueing them with the next target response. However, the SRT task only records keypress response times to a cued target, and thus it cannot reveal the full time‐course of motion, including predictive movements. This paper describes a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  9.  25
    Predictive Movements and Human Reinforcement Learning of Sequential Action.Roy de Kleijn, George Kachergis & Bernhard Hommel - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S3):783-808.
    Sequential action makes up the bulk of human daily activity, and yet much remains unknown about how people learn such actions. In one motor learning paradigm, the serial reaction time (SRT) task, people are taught a consistent sequence of button presses by cueing them with the next target response. However, the SRT task only records keypress response times to a cued target, and thus it cannot reveal the full time‐course of motion, including predictive movements. This paper describes a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10.  51
    Motor memory: Consolidation–based enhancement effect revisited.Julien Doyon, Julie Carrier, Alain Simard, Abdallah Hadj Tahar, Amélie Morin, Habib Benali & Leslie G. Ungerleider - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):68-69.
    Following Karni's seminal work, Walker and other researchers have recently provided gradually convincing evidence that sleep is critical for the consolidation-based enhancement (CBE) of motor sequence learning. Studies in our laboratory using a motor adaptation paradigm, however, show that CBE can also occur after the simple passage of time, suggesting that sleep effects on memory consolidation are task-related, and possibly dependent on anatomically dissociable circuits.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11. Implicit Learning, Bilingualism, and Dyslexia: Insights From a Study Assessing AGL With a Modified Simon Task.Maria Vender, Diego Gabriel Krivochen, Beth Phillips, Douglas Saddy & Denis Delfitto - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    This paper presents an experimental study investigating artificial grammar learning (AGL) in monolingual and bilingual children, with and without dyslexia, using an original methodology. We administered a serial reaction time (SRT) task, in the form of a modified Simon task, in which the sequence of the stimuli was manipulated according to the rules of a simple Lindenmayer grammar (more specifically, a Fibonacci grammar). By ensuring that the subjects focused on the correct response execution at the motor stage (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  12. Learning to see after early and extended blindness: A scoping review.Eloise May, Proscovia Arach, Elizabeth Kishiki, Robert Geneau, Goro Maehara, Mahadeo Sukhai & Lisa M. Hamm - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    PurposeIf an individual has been blind since birth due to a treatable eye condition, ocular treatment is urgent. Even a brief period of visual deprivation can alter the development of the visual system. The goal of our structured scoping review was to understand how we might better support children with delayed access to ocular treatment for blinding conditions.MethodWe searched MEDLINE, Embase and Global Health for peer-reviewed publications that described the impact of early and extended bilateral visual deprivation.ResultsOf 551 reports independently (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  42
    What connectionist models learn: Learning and representation in connectionist networks.Stephen José Hanson & David J. Burr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):471-489.
    Connectionist models provide a promising alternative to the traditional computational approach that has for several decades dominated cognitive science and artificial intelligence, although the nature of connectionist models and their relation to symbol processing remains controversial. Connectionist models can be characterized by three general computational features: distinct layers of interconnected units, recursive rules for updating the strengths of the connections during learning, and “simple” homogeneous computing elements. Using just these three features one can construct surprisingly elegant and powerful (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  14.  20
    Complex Motor Learning and Police Training: Applied, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives.Paula M. Di Nota & Juha-Matti Huhta - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  15.  14
    Modulating Motor Learning through Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation: An Integrative View.Claudia Ammann, Danny Spampinato & Javier Márquez-Ruiz - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  16.  9
    Postrest motor learning performance as a function of degree of learning.John C. Jahnke - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (6):605.
  17.  5
    Motor Learning in Response to Different Experimental Pain Models Among Healthy Individuals: A Systematic Review.Mohammad Izadi, Sae Franklin, Marianna Bellafiore & David W. Franklin - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Learning new movement patterns is a normal part of daily life, but of critical importance in both sport and rehabilitation. A major question is how different sensory signals are integrated together to give rise to motor adaptation and learning. More specifically, there is growing evidence that pain can give rise to alterations in the learning process. Despite a number of studies investigating the role of pain on the learning process, there is still no systematic review (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  25
    Motor learning models.Daniel M. Wolpert & Zoubin Ghahramani - 2003 - In L. Nadel (ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Nature Publishing Group.
  19.  18
    Motor learning and synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum.Richard F. Thompson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):475-477.
    For reasons I have never understood, some students of the cerebellum have been unwilling to accept the now overwhelming evidence that the cerebellum exhibits lasting synaptic plasticity and plays an essential role in some forms of learning and memory. With a few exceptions (e.g., target article by SIMPSON et al.) this is no longer the case, as is clear in the excellent target articles on cerebellar LTD and the excellent target review by HOUK et al. [CRÉPEL et al.; HOUR (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  11
    Motor Learning Improves the Stability of Large-Scale Brain Connectivity Pattern.Mengxia Yu, Haoming Song, Jialin Huang, Yiying Song & Jia Liu - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  21. Motor learning and memory for visually guided reaching.R. Shadmehr & S. P. Wise - 2004 - In Michael S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences Iii. MIT Press. pp. 353--375.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  30
    Toddlers Using Tablets: They Engage, Play, and Learn.Mary L. Courage, Lynn M. Frizzell, Colin S. Walsh & Megan Smith - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Although very young children have unprecedented access to touchscreen devices, there is limited research on how successfully they operate these devices for play and learning. For infants and toddlers, whose cognitive, fine motor, and executive functions are immature, several basic questions are significant: Can they operate a tablet purposefully to achieve a goal? Can they acquire operating skills and learn new information from commercially available apps? Do individual differences in executive functioning predict success in using and learning (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  17
    Simple concept learning as a function of intralist generalization.Marian Hooper Baum - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (2):89.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  9
    Reminiscence in motor learning as a function of prerest distribution of practice.Benjamin H. Pubols - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (3):155.
  25.  16
    Performance decrement in a simple motor task before and after a single rest.Edward A. Bilodeau - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 43 (5):381.
  26.  15
    Retroaction and gains in motor learning: II. Sex differences, and a further analysis of gains.C. E. Buxton & D. A. Grant - 1939 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 25 (2):198.
  27.  50
    Response feedback and motor learning.Jack A. Adams, Ernest T. Goetz & Phillip H. Marshall - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (3):391.
  28.  17
    Reminiscence in motor learning as a function of length of interpolated rest.Gregory A. Kimble & Betty R. Horenstein - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (3):239.
  29.  18
    Transfer in motor learning as a function of degree of first-task learning and inter-task similarity.Carl P. Duncan - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 (1):1.
  30.  45
    Marginally perceptible outcome feedback, motor learning and implicit processes.Rich S. W. Masters, Jon P. Maxwell & Frank F. Eves - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (3):639-645.
    Participants struck 500 golf balls to a concealed target. Outcome feedback was presented at the subjective or objective threshold of awareness of each participant or at a supraliminal threshold. Participants who received fully perceptible feedback learned to strike the ball onto the target, as did participants who received feedback that was only marginally perceptible . Participants who received feedback that was not perceptible showed no learning. Upon transfer to a condition in which the target was unconcealed, performance increased in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  31.  15
    Performance and reminiscence in motor learning as a function of the degree of distribution of practice.Gregory A. Kimble - 1949 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 39 (4):500.
  32.  28
    Transfer of training in motor learning as a function of distribution of practice.Victor E. Montgomery - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 46 (6):440.
  33.  7
    Mechanisms of Human Motor Learning Do Not Function Independently.Amanda S. Therrien & Aaron L. Wong - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Human motor learning is governed by a suite of interacting mechanisms each one of which modifies behavior in distinct ways and rely on different neural circuits. In recent years, much attention has been given to one type of motor learning, called motor adaptation. Here, the field has generally focused on the interactions of three mechanisms: sensory prediction error SPE-driven, explicit, and reinforcement learning. Studies of these mechanisms have largely treated them as modular, aiming to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  12
    Distributed practice in motor learning: score changes within and between daily sessions.E. R. Hilgard & M. B. Smith - 1942 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 30 (2):136.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  18
    Intention in motor learning.S. Sanderson - 1929 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 12 (6):463.
  36.  15
    Distributed practice in motor learning: progressively increasing and decreasing rests.Barbara S. Cook & Ernest R. Hilgard - 1949 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 39 (2):169.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  8
    Cognitive and Motor Learning in Internally-Guided Motor Skills.Krishn Bera, Anuj Shukla & Raju S. Bapi - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Several canonical experimental paradigms have been proposed to study the typical behavioral phenomenon and the nature of learning in sequential keypress tasks. A characteristic feature of most paradigms is that they are representative of externally-specified sequencing—motor tasks where the environment or task paradigm extrinsically provides the sequence of stimuli, i.e., the responses are stimulus-driven. Previous studies utilizing such canonical paradigms have largely overlooked the learning behaviors in a more realistic class of motor tasks that involve internally-guided (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  21
    Time scales in motor learning and development.Karl M. Newell, Yeou-Teh Liu & Gottfried Mayer-Kress - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (1):57-82.
  39.  12
    Perceptual-Cognitive Changes During Motor Learning: The Influence of Mental and Physical Practice on Mental Representation, Gaze Behavior, and Performance of a Complex Action.Cornelia Frank, William M. Land & Thomas Schack - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  40.  17
    Retroaction and gains in motor learning: I. Similarity of interpolated task as a factor in gains.C. E. Buxton & C. E. Henry - 1939 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 25 (1):1.
  41.  10
    Neuroplasticity in Motor Learning Under Variable and Constant Practice Conditions—Protocol of Randomized Controlled Trial.Stanisław H. Czyż, Jarosław Marusiak, Patrícia Klobušiaková, Zuzana Sajdlová & Irena Rektorová - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    BackgroundThere is numerous literature on mechanisms underlying variability of practice advantages. Literature includes both behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Unfortunately, no studies are focusing on practice in constant conditions to the best of our knowledge. Hence it is essential to assess possible differences in mechanisms of neuroplasticity between constant vs. variable practice conditions. The primary objectives of the study described in this protocol will be: to determine the brain’s structural and functional changes following constant and variable practice conditions in motor (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  12
    Retention of transfer in motor learning after twenty-four hours and after fourteen months.Carl P. Duncan & Benton J. Underwood - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 46 (6):445.
  43.  83
    Transfer of Motor Learning Is More Pronounced in Proximal Compared to Distal Effectors in Upper Extremities.Tore K. Aune, Morten A. Aune, Rolf P. Ingvaldsen & Beatrix Vereijken - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  44.  20
    Understanding Self-Controlled Motor Learning Protocols through the Self-Determination Theory.Elizabeth A. Sanli, Jae T. Patterson, Steven R. Bray & Timothy D. Lee - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  45.  13
    Retention in motor learning as a function of amount of practice and rest.John C. Jahnke - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (3):270.
  46.  12
    Self-Controlled Feedback Facilitates Motor Learning in Both High and Low Activity Individuals.Jeffrey T. Fairbrother, David D. Laughlin & Timothy V. Nguyen - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  47.  24
    Reminiscence and forgetting in motor learning after extended rest intervals.John C. Jahnke & Carl P. Duncan - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 52 (5):273.
  48.  7
    Brain Activity Reveals Multiple Motor-Learning Mechanisms in a Real-World Task.Shlomi Haar & A. Aldo Faisal - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  49.  22
    Social Cues Alter Implicit Motor Learning in a Serial Reaction Time Task.Alexander Geiger, Axel Cleeremans, Gary Bente & Kai Vogeley - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  50.  6
    Commentary: Complex Motor Learning and Police Training: Applied, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives.Mario S. Staller & Swen Körner - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 1000